Dynamic Luminance-Gradient Effect

Dynamic Luminance-Gradient Effect

This illusion is an example of the Dynamic Luminance-Gradient Effect.

To make this work, you’ll need to stare at the white spot in the center and then move your face closer to the screen. When you move forward, the white spot will expand and increase in luminescence.

If you are interested in learning more about the Dynamic Luminance-Gradient Effect, scroll down to read more about it.

Dynamic Luminance-Gradient Effect


Table of Contents

What is the Dynamic Luminance-Gradient Effect?

The Dynamic Luminance-Gradient Effect is a perceptual phenomenon in which a static image appears to have motion or movement due to the manipulation of luminance gradients within the image. This effect is achieved by creating a pattern of luminance gradients that are organized in a specific way to produce the illusion of motion.

For example, an image of parallel lines with alternating light and dark stripes can create the impression of movement when the lines are tilted or moved in a particular direction. This effect is thought to be caused by the visual system’s sensitivity to changes in luminance contrast, which is a property of how the brightness of different parts of an image compare to each other.

The Dynamic Luminance-Gradient Effect has been studied extensively in the fields of psychology and neuroscience, as it can provide insights into how the brain processes visual information and creates the perception of motion. It is also commonly used in art and design to create visual interest and the illusion of movement in static images

How does the Dynamic Luminance-Gradient Effect Work?

The Dynamic Luminance-Gradient Effect works by taking advantage of the human visual system’s sensitivity to changes in luminance contrast. Our visual system is highly attuned to differences in brightness, and it uses these differences to identify and track objects in our environment.

When we look at an image that contains luminance gradients organized in a specific way, our visual system interprets these gradients as indicating motion. This is because our brains are wired to assume that changes in brightness over time are caused by objects moving in space.

The specific mechanisms by which the visual system processes luminance gradients and creates the illusion of motion are still not fully understood, and researchers continue to study this phenomenon to gain insights into how the brain processes visual information. However, it is clear that the Dynamic Luminance-Gradient Effect is a powerful tool for creating visual interest and the impression of movement in static images.

Some Similar Illusions

There are several similar illusions to the Dynamic Luminance-Gradient Effect that also rely on the visual system’s sensitivity to contrast and motion. Here are a few examples:

  1. Motion Aftereffect (MAE): This is an illusion in which a stationary object appears to be moving after the viewer has been exposed to a moving object for an extended period of time. This effect is thought to be caused by neurons in the visual system adapting to the constant motion, which leads to a temporary imbalance in the perception of motion.
  2. Op Art: Op art, short for optical art, is a style of art that creates the illusion of movement or three-dimensionality through the use of patterns and contrast. This style often features repeating geometric shapes or lines that are arranged in a way that creates a visual vibration or pulsation.
  3. The Pinna-Brelstaff illusion: This is an illusion in which a static image appears to rotate when the viewer moves towards or away from it. This effect is achieved by combining opposing diagonal patterns that create an impression of motion in the direction of the viewer’s movement.
  4. The Hermann Grid illusion: This is an illusion in which grey dots appear at the intersections of a grid of black lines on a white background. This effect is caused by the way that the visual system processes contrast, and it can be enhanced by increasing the size of the grid or by adjusting the brightness of the dots and background.

Discovery of the Dynamic Luminance-Gradient Effect

The Dynamic Luminance-Gradient Effect has been studied by many researchers in the fields of psychology, neuroscience, and visual perception, and it is not attributed to any one specific individual as its discoverer.

However, the phenomenon has been documented in scientific literature since the early 20th century. For example, the artist and researcher Josef Albers described the effect in his book “Interaction of Color” in 1963, and it has since been studied extensively in the context of visual perception and neuroscience.

Overall, the Dynamic Luminance-Gradient Effect is a well-established perceptual phenomenon that has been observed and studied by many researchers over the year


References and Resources

Check out our complete list of illusions.

Illusory Sunrise and Sunset

Illusory Sunrise and Sunset

An Illusory Sunrise and Sunset creates an awe inspiring view

An illusory sunrise or sunset, also known as a “false sunrise” or “false sunset,” is a phenomenon that can occur when the sun is still below the horizon but its rays are refracted or bent by the Earth’s atmosphere. This causes the sun’s light to be visible above the horizon, creating the appearance of a sunrise or sunset.

Check out these beautiful views of Illusory Sunrise and Sunsets, and if you are interested in learning more about Illusory Sunrises and Sunsets, scroll down to read more about them.

Illusory Sunrise and Sunset
Illusory Sunrise and Sunset
Illusory Sunrise and Sunset
Illusory Sunrise and Sunset


Table of Contents

What are Illusory Sunrises and Sunsets?

An illusory sunrise or sunset, also known as a “false sunrise” or “false sunset,” is a phenomenon that can occur when the sun is still below the horizon but its rays are refracted or bent by the Earth’s atmosphere. This causes the sun’s light to be visible above the horizon, creating the appearance of a sunrise or sunset.

The most common type of illusory sunrise or sunset is called a “green flash,” which is a brief burst of green light that can be seen just as the sun appears or disappears below the horizon. This occurs when the atmosphere refracts the sun’s light, separating it into different colors and causing the green light to be visible for a split second.

Illusory sunrises and sunsets are most commonly seen near the ocean, where the horizon is unobstructed, and when the atmosphere is clear and stable. They are rare and can be difficult to observe, but they are considered a beautiful and fascinating natural phenomenon.

How do Illusory Sunrises and Sunsets Work?

Illusory sunrises and sunsets, also known as false sunsets and false sunrises, occur when the sun’s light is refracted or bent by the Earth’s atmosphere. The atmosphere is composed of different layers of air with varying densities and temperatures, and these layers can cause the sun’s rays to bend and scatter in different directions.

When the sun is near the horizon, its light passes through a larger portion of the atmosphere, and this causes more bending of the light. The bending is called refraction, and it causes the light to be spread out into different colors, similar to how light is separated into a rainbow by a prism. This refraction of light can cause the sun to appear to be higher or lower than it actually is, and can even create the appearance of multiple suns or a distorted sun shape.

The most common type of illusory sunrise or sunset is called a green flash. This occurs when the atmosphere separates the sun’s light into different colors, and the green light becomes visible for a brief moment just as the sun disappears below the horizon.

Overall, illusory sunrises and sunsets are a result of the bending and scattering of light by the Earth’s atmosphere, and they are a fascinating and beautiful natural phenomenon.

Some Similar Illusions

There are several other illusions that involve the bending or refraction of light, which are similar to illusory sunrises and sunsets. Here are a few examples:

  1. Fata Morgana: This is a complex form of mirage, caused by the refraction of light through different layers of the atmosphere. It can create the illusion of castles, ships, or even entire cities floating in the sky.
  2. Mirage: A mirage is an optical illusion caused by the bending of light as it passes through air layers with different temperatures. It can make objects appear to be displaced or distorted.
  3. Rainbow: A rainbow is an optical phenomenon that is caused by the refraction, reflection, and dispersion of light in water droplets. It creates a colorful arc in the sky, often seen after a rainstorm.
  4. Twilight: Twilight is the period of time before sunrise or after sunset when the sky is illuminated by indirect sunlight that is scattered in the upper atmosphere. It can create colorful and dramatic skies, similar to those seen during a sunrise or sunset.
  5. Halo: A halo is a ring of light that surrounds the sun or moon, caused by the refraction of light through ice crystals in the atmosphere. It can create a striking and ethereal visual effect.

These optical illusions are all caused by the bending or refraction of light, and they can create stunning and sometimes surreal visual experiences.

Discovery of the Illusory Sunrises and Sunsets

The phenomenon of illusory sunrises and sunsets has been observed for centuries, and the underlying science of atmospheric refraction and bending of light has been studied and explained by many scientists over the years.

One of the earliest documented explanations of atmospheric refraction was provided by the ancient Greek philosopher Aristotle, who observed that the apparent position of stars near the horizon was slightly higher than their true position, and he hypothesized that this was due to the bending of light in the Earth’s atmosphere.

In the modern era, several scientists have made significant contributions to our understanding of atmospheric refraction and the formation of illusory sunrises and sunsets. One of the most notable figures is French astronomer Pierre Bouguer, who in the early 18th century conducted experiments and wrote extensively about atmospheric refraction and its effects on celestial observations.

Another important figure in the study of atmospheric optics is English scientist Thomas Young, who in the early 19th century proposed a wave theory of light that helped to explain many optical phenomena, including the bending of light in the atmosphere.

Today, the science of atmospheric optics is a well-established field of study, and scientists continue to investigate and refine our understanding of the complex ways in which light interacts with the Earth’s atmosphere.


References and Resources

Check out our complete list of illusions.

Chromatic Adaptation Illusion

chromatic adaptation illusion

This Chromatic Adaptation Illusion allows you to see a black and white image in full color. How?

Stare intently at the brightly colored GIF. If you continue to stare hard, you’ll be able to see the following Black & white image in full color.

If you are interested in learning more about the Chromatic Adaptation Illusion and how it works, scroll down to read more about it.

chromatic adaptation illusion
Stare intently at the brightly colored GIF. If you continue to stare hard, you’ll be able to see the following Black & white image in full color.


Table of Contents

What is the Chromatic Adaptation Illusion?

Chromatic adaptation illusion refers to the phenomenon where the colors of an object appear to change when they are viewed under different lighting conditions. This illusion occurs because the human visual system adjusts its sensitivity to different colors based on the ambient lighting.

For example, if you look at a white piece of paper under bright sunlight, it will appear to be white. However, if you look at the same piece of paper under a dimmer light, such as candlelight, it may appear to have a yellowish tint. This is because the lighting conditions have changed, and your visual system has adapted to the new lighting by adjusting the sensitivity of your color receptors.

The chromatic adaptation illusion can also be demonstrated using color patches. If you view a colored patch under one type of lighting and then view it under another type of lighting, the patch will appear to change color. This illusion is particularly pronounced with colors that are close to the edge of the visible spectrum, such as blue and violet.

Overall, the chromatic adaptation illusion demonstrates the remarkable ability of the human visual system to adjust to changes in the environment, and highlights the importance of considering the effects of lighting conditions when interpreting color perception.

How does the Chromatic Adaptation Illusion Work?

The chromatic adaptation illusion occurs because our visual system is constantly adapting to changes in the ambient lighting. When we view an object, the light that reflects off of it enters our eyes and is processed by special cells called photoreceptors in the retina. These photoreceptors are responsible for detecting color and transmitting that information to the brain.

However, the sensitivity of these photoreceptors can vary depending on the lighting conditions. For example, in bright sunlight, the photoreceptors are more sensitive to blue light, while in dim lighting, they are more sensitive to red light. This is because the photoreceptors adjust their sensitivity to different wavelengths of light based on the overall color of the light that is present.

When we view an object under different lighting conditions, our visual system adjusts the sensitivity of the photoreceptors accordingly. This adjustment process is known as chromatic adaptation. The result is that the perceived color of the object changes depending on the lighting conditions.

For example, if we view a white object under a bright blue light, our visual system adapts to the blue light by reducing the sensitivity of the photoreceptors that are most sensitive to blue light. This makes the object appear white. However, if we view the same object under a dim red light, our visual system adapts to the red light by reducing the sensitivity of the photoreceptors that are most sensitive to green light. This makes the object appear reddish.

In summary, the chromatic adaptation illusion occurs because our visual system adjusts the sensitivity of the photoreceptors based on the overall color of the ambient lighting, which can cause the perceived color of an object to change depending on the lighting conditions.

Some Similar Illusions

There are many other types of illusions similar to the chromatic adaptation illusion, which involve changes in perception due to changes in the surrounding environment. Here are some examples:

  1. The brightness illusion: This illusion occurs when the same color appears brighter or dimmer depending on the surrounding colors. For example, a gray square may appear darker when surrounded by lighter shades, and lighter when surrounded by darker shades.
  2. The contrast illusion: This illusion occurs when the perceived contrast of an object is influenced by the colors or patterns around it. For example, a gray bar may appear darker against a light background and lighter against a dark background.
  3. The color assimilation illusion: This illusion occurs when a color appears to “spread” into neighboring areas of the same or similar colors. For example, a red dot surrounded by a yellow ring may appear to have a slightly orange tint due to the influence of the surrounding yellow.
  4. The size illusion: This illusion occurs when the perceived size of an object is influenced by the surrounding context. For example, a circle surrounded by smaller circles may appear larger than the same circle surrounded by larger circles.
  5. The motion aftereffect illusion: This illusion occurs when a stationary object appears to move in the opposite direction after viewing a moving object. For example, after watching a spinning spiral, a stationary object may appear to spin in the opposite direction.

These illusions, like the chromatic adaptation illusion, demonstrate the complex ways in which our perception of the world is influenced by our surrounding environment and the workings of the visual system.

Discovery of the Chromatic Adaptation Illusion

The chromatic adaptation illusion is a well-known phenomenon in the field of color perception, and its discovery cannot be attributed to a single individual or moment in history. Rather, it has been studied and documented by many researchers over the years.

One of the earliest descriptions of chromatic adaptation was provided by the German physiologist Ewald Hering in the late 19th century. Hering proposed that the visual system adapts to different colors by adjusting the sensitivity of the three types of color receptors in the retina. This idea has since been supported by many studies in vision science.

Another important figure in the study of chromatic adaptation was the American psychologist Edwin H. Land, who is best known for his invention of instant photography. Land conducted numerous experiments on color vision and chromatic adaptation in the mid-20th century, and his work helped to establish the principles of color constancy and color adaptation that are still used today.

Since then, many researchers in vision science and related fields have contributed to our understanding of the chromatic adaptation illusion, including David Brainard, Michael Webster, and Andrew Stockman, among many others. The study of color perception remains an active area of research, and new insights into the workings of the visual system continue to be discovered.


References and Resources

Check out our complete list of illusions.

Super Cool Illusory Motion

Illusory Motion

This Super Cool Illusory Motion example is not a GIF. It is a completely static image.

Illusory motion is a perceptual phenomenon in which a stationary image appears to be moving. It occurs when visual cues in the image trick the brain into perceiving motion, even though there is no actual movement taking place. If you are interested in learning more, scroll down to read more about it.

Also check out these cool examples of illusory motion: Amazing Colorful Illusory Motion and Black and White Illusory Motion and Fun Circles Illusory Motion and Illusory Motion

Illusory Motion


Table of Contents

What is Illusory Motion?

Illusory motion is a type of optical illusion in which stationary images or patterns appear to be moving. This can occur in a variety of ways, such as through the use of patterns with alternating colors or shapes that create the illusion of motion, or by presenting a series of static images in rapid succession to create the perception of motion.

One well-known example of an illusory motion effect is the “rotating snakes” illusion, in which a series of static black-and-white shapes appear to be rotating in a continuous, fluid motion. This illusion is created by using patterns with specific shapes and contrasts that stimulate the brain’s motion-sensitive neurons and create the perception of movement, even though the image itself is not actually moving.

Other examples of illusory motion include the “scintillating grid” illusion, in which the intersections of a grid pattern appear to be flashing or moving, and the “phi phenomenon,” in which a series of static lights flashing in sequence create the illusion of motion.

Illusory motion can be a fascinating and captivating experience, and it has been the subject of much research in the fields of visual perception and neuroscience. Scientists continue to study the underlying mechanisms of illusory motion and other visual illusions in order to better understand how the brain processes visual information and creates our subjective experience of the world around us.

How does Illusory Motion Work?

Illusory motion is caused by the brain’s interpretation of visual information that is presented in a particular way. Different illusory motion effects may be created by different types of visual stimuli, but they all involve the brain perceiving motion where there is none.

One explanation for illusory motion is that it is caused by the brain’s motion-sensitive neurons responding to certain visual patterns or stimuli in a way that creates the perception of motion. These neurons, located in an area of the brain called the visual cortex, are responsible for processing information about motion and spatial relationships in the visual field. When presented with certain visual patterns or stimuli, these neurons can become activated in a way that creates the illusion of motion.

Another explanation is that illusory motion is a result of the brain’s tendency to fill in missing information in order to create a complete and coherent visual scene. When presented with incomplete or ambiguous visual information, the brain may “fill in the gaps” in a way that creates the perception of motion or movement.

In either case, illusory motion is a result of the brain’s complex processing of visual information, and it is influenced by a variety of factors, including the properties of the visual stimuli, the context in which they are presented, and individual differences in perception and interpretation.

Some Similar Illusions

There are many different illusory motion illusions, each created by specific patterns or stimuli that trick the brain into perceiving motion where there is none. Here are some examples of illusory motion illusions:

  1. Rotating snakes illusion: This illusion consists of a pattern of overlapping circles and curves that create the perception of continuous motion, as if the image is rotating in a circular motion.
  2. Motion aftereffect illusion: This illusion occurs when a person views a moving stimulus for a prolonged period of time, and then looks at a stationary object. The stationary object will appear to be moving in the opposite direction of the original stimulus.
  3. Autokinetic effect: This illusion occurs when a stationary point of light is viewed in a dark room for a prolonged period of time. The light will appear to move or “drift” even though it is stationary.
  4. Peripheral drift illusion: This illusion consists of a pattern of intersecting circles and lines that create the perception of motion at the periphery of the visual field.
  5. Barber pole illusion: This illusion consists of a rotating spiral pattern of alternating red and white stripes, which create the perception of upward motion even though the pattern itself is rotating.
  6. Wagon wheel illusion: This illusion occurs when a wheel appears to be rotating in the opposite direction of its true motion, due to the interaction between the frequency of the spokes and the frame rate of the video camera.

These are just a few examples of the many illusory motion effects that have been discovered and studied by researchers in the field of visual perception. Each of these illusions demonstrates the brain’s remarkable ability to create the perception of motion and movement, even in the absence of actual movement.

Discovery of the Illusory Motion

Illusory motion has been known and studied by scientists and artists for centuries, but it is difficult to attribute its discovery or popularization to any single individual or group. The use of visual patterns and stimuli to create the illusion of motion has been explored in various forms of art, such as Op Art and Kinetic Art, and in scientific research on visual perception and neuroscience.

One of the earliest recorded examples of an illusory motion effect is the Zoetrope, a pre-cinematic device invented in the early 19th century that uses a sequence of static images to create the illusion of motion. Other early examples of illusory motion effects can be found in ancient Greek and Roman art, such as the use of mosaic patterns to create the impression of motion and depth.

In more recent times, scientists and artists have continued to explore and experiment with the use of visual illusions to create the perception of motion. Artists such as Bridget Riley and Victor Vasarely are known for their use of geometric patterns and shapes to create illusory motion effects, while scientists have used illusory motion as a tool for studying the brain’s processing of visual information.

Overall, illusory motion is a phenomenon that has been studied and appreciated by many different people throughout history, and it continues to inspire new forms of artistic and scientific exploration.


References and Resources Illusory Motion

In addition to this supercool Illusory Motion example, check out our complete list of illusions.

Color Afterimage Illusions

Color Afterimage Illusion

These Color Afterimage illusions occur when an image continues to appear in our visual field after we stop looking at it.

To give these color afterimage illusions a try, stare at each of the designs for 30 seconds and then stare at white surface. You’ll end up seeing the “opposite” or “complimentary” colors.

If you are interested in learning more about Color Afterimage Illusions, scroll down to read more about them it.

Color Afterimage Illusion
Stare at this for 30 seconds and then look at a white surface. You should see the familiar red, white, and blue because they complementary colors of cyan, black, and yellow.
Color Afterimage Illusion
Stare at this for 30 seconds and then look at a white surface. You should see the familiar green, white, and red of the Italian Flag.
Color Afterimage Illusion
Stare at the white dot for 30 seconds and then close your eyes. You should see a cyan circle
Color Afterimage Illusion
Color Afterimage Illusion
Stare at the center of a single circle for 30 seconds then divert to a white surface. Experiment with the different colors.


Table of Contents

What are Color Afterimage Illusions?

Color afterimages are a type of afterimage that occurs when we look at a colored object or image for a prolonged period of time, and then look away to a neutral background. The afterimage that we see appears in the complementary color to the original color of the object or image.

For example, if we stare at a red object for a period of time, the afterimage that we see when we look away will appear in green, which is the complementary color of red. Similarly, if we stare at a green object, the afterimage that we see will appear in red. This phenomenon is known as “negative afterimage” or “complementary afterimage”.

The reason for this phenomenon is that the photoreceptor cells in our eyes that detect color are most sensitive to certain wavelengths of light. When we stare at a colored object for a period of time, these photoreceptor cells become fatigued, and their sensitivity to the color in question decreases. When we then look away to a neutral background, the photoreceptor cells that were not fatigued are still sensitive, and they respond strongly to the complementary color, creating the illusion of a color afterimage.

Color afterimages can be a fascinating and beautiful visual experience, and they have been used in art and design to create interesting and striking visual effects. However, they can also be a useful tool for researchers studying visual perception and the mechanisms of color vision.

How do Color Afterimage Illusions Work?

Color afterimages are a type of optical illusion that occurs when you look at a brightly colored object for a period of time and then look away, only to see a ghostly image of the object in a different color. These afterimages are created by the way that our eyes and brain process color information.

When we look at a brightly colored object, the light from that object stimulates specialized cells in our eyes called cone cells. These cone cells are responsible for detecting different colors, and they send signals to our brain that help us perceive the color of the object.

However, when we look at a brightly colored object for a period of time, these cone cells can become fatigued or “adapted” to the color of the object. This means that they become less sensitive to that color over time, and when we look away from the object, they continue to send signals to our brain that create the perception of the opposite or complementary color.

For example, if you stare at a red object for a period of time and then look away, you may see a ghostly image of the object in green. This is because green is the complementary color to red, and when the cone cells that are responsible for detecting red become fatigued, they send signals to the brain that create the perception of green.

Overall, color afterimages are a fascinating example of how our eyes and brain process color information, and they can be used to study the mechanisms of perception and cognition.

Some Similar Illusions

There are several types of illusions that are similar to color afterimage illusions, as they involve the way our eyes and brain process visual information. Some examples include:

  1. Motion aftereffect illusion: This illusion occurs when we view a moving object for a prolonged period of time, and then look at a stationary object. The stationary object appears to be moving in the opposite direction to the original moving object.
  2. Contrast illusion: This illusion occurs when we view a dark object against a light background, and then view a light object against a dark background. The light object appears brighter and more intense than it actually is, while the dark object appears darker and less intense.
  3. Color adaptation illusion: This illusion occurs when we view a colored object for a prolonged period of time, and then look at a neutral colored object. The neutral object appears to have a tint of the complementary color to the original colored object.
  4. Troxler’s fading illusion: This illusion occurs when we stare at a fixed point on a stationary image, and the other parts of the image begin to fade away over time. This happens because our brain filters out visual information that is not changing, allowing us to focus on important visual stimuli.

Overall, these illusions demonstrate the complex and dynamic ways in which our eyes and brain process visual information, and they continue to fascinate researchers and laypeople alike.

Discovery of Color Afterimage Illusions

The phenomenon of color afterimages has been known for centuries, and it is likely that many ancient cultures observed and discussed the phenomenon. However, the scientific study of afterimages and their underlying mechanisms began in the 19th century, with the work of several researchers.

One of the first researchers to study afterimages was a German physicist named Georg Christoph Lichtenberg, who conducted experiments in the late 18th century to investigate the nature of visual afterimages. Another early researcher in this field was the French physiologist Etienne-Jules Marey, who published a series of studies on visual perception and afterimages in the late 19th century.

However, it was the German physicist Hermann von Helmholtz who made some of the most important contributions to the study of afterimages in the 19th century. Helmholtz conducted numerous experiments to investigate the mechanisms of afterimages, and his work laid the foundation for much of the modern understanding of visual perception.

Today, afterimages remain a topic of ongoing research and fascination among scientists, psychologists, and artists alike.


References and Resources Color Afterimage Illusions

In addition to Color Afterimage Illusions, please check out our complete list of illusions.

Pulsating Flower Illusions

Pulsating Flower Illusions

Check out these Pulsating Flower Illusions that create a sense of movement or pulsation in the visual field, even when there is no actual motion occurring. These types illusions can be caused by a variety of visual stimuli, including patterns, colors, and shapes.

If you are interested in learning more about Pulsating Flower Illusions, scroll down to read more about them. Also, check out these cool Pulsating Illusions.

Pulsating Flower Illusions
Pulsating Flower Illusions
Pulsating Flower Illusions


Table of Contents

What are Pulsating Flower Illusions?

Pulsating Flower Illusions that create a sense of movement or pulsation in the visual field, even when there is no actual motion occurring. These types illusions can be caused by a variety of visual stimuli, including patterns, colors, and shapes. They are usually caused by the way our brains process visual information.

Our brains constantly receive and process sensory information from our environment, including visual information from our eyes. When we look at an object or scene, our brain uses a variety of processing mechanisms to interpret the visual information and create a perception of what we are seeing.

In the case of pulsating flower illusions, the visual information is processed in a way that creates a rhythmic, pulsing effect. This can happen for a variety of reasons, including the way that certain visual elements in the illusion interact with each other, or the way that our brain processes and integrates different visual inputs.

For example, one common type of pulsating illusion is the “grid illusion,” where a grid of intersecting lines appears to pulsate and move. This effect is thought to be caused by the way that the intersections between the lines create contrasting areas of light and dark, which our brain interprets as moving or pulsing.

Overall, these illusions are a fascinating example of how our brains process visual information and can be used to study the mechanisms of perception and cognition.

How do Pulsating Flower Illusions Work?

Pulsating flower illusions work by creating a sense of movement or pulsation in the visual field, even when there is no actual motion occurring. These illusions can be caused by a variety of visual stimuli, including patterns, colors, and shapes.

One common example of a pulsating illusion is the “Neon Color Spreading” illusion, where a brightly colored object appears to pulsate and spread color to its surroundings. This illusion is created by the way that our brain processes visual information about color and brightness. When two contrasting colors, such as red and green, are placed next to each other, they create a visual contrast that our brain interprets as pulsing or moving.

Another example is the “Grid Illusion,” where a grid of intersecting lines appears to pulsate and move. This illusion is caused by the way that the intersections between the lines create contrasting areas of light and dark, which our brain interprets as moving or pulsing.

In general, pulsating flower illusions are thought to be caused by the way that our brain processes and integrates visual information. By studying these illusions, scientists can gain insights into the mechanisms of perception and cognition, and how our brains create our sense of reality.

Some Similar Illusions

There are many different types of illusions that are similar to pulsating flower illusions in that they create a sense of movement or distortion in the visual field. Here are a few examples:

  1. Motion aftereffect illusion: This illusion occurs when you stare at a moving image for a period of time and then look at a stationary object, which appears to move in the opposite direction. This effect is caused by the way that our brain adapts to the motion of the original image and then overcompensates when we look at the stationary object.
  2. Pinna-Brelstaff illusion: This illusion occurs when you look at a spiraling image that is also rotating. The image appears to move in the opposite direction of the rotation, creating a sense of disorientation and distortion.
  3. Ames room illusion: In this illusion, a room is constructed with one corner closer to the viewer than the other, creating the illusion of a trapezoidal shape. When people enter the room, they appear to shrink or grow in size as they move from one corner to the other. This effect is caused by the way that our brain processes visual depth and perspective.
  4. Ponzo illusion: This illusion occurs when two lines of the same length are placed in a converging pattern with two diagonal lines. The line that is closer to the converging point appears longer, even though it is actually the same length as the other line. This effect is caused by the way that our brain interprets visual cues such as perspective and depth.

Overall, illusions are a fascinating way to explore how our brain processes visual information and constructs our perception of reality.

Discovery of Pulsating Flower Illusions

Pulsating flower illusions, like many types of visual illusions, have been studied and documented by a number of scientists and researchers over the years. It’s difficult to attribute the discovery or popularization of pulsating illusions to any one person or group.

One of the earliest documented examples of a pulsating illusion is the “phi phenomenon,” which was first described by the psychologist Max Wertheimer in 1912. The phi phenomenon occurs when two or more visual stimuli are presented in rapid succession, creating the illusion of movement or pulsation.

Since then, many other researchers have studied pulsating illusions and related phenomena, including the ways that our brain processes visual information and creates the perception of movement and motion. Some notable contributors to this field include the psychologists Richard Gregory, Edward Adelson, and Akiyoshi Kitaoka, among others.

Today, pulsating flower illusions continue to be a fascinating area of study for researchers in fields like psychology, neuroscience, and cognitive science, and new discoveries are constantly being made about the ways that our brains interpret and process visual information.


References and Resources – Pulsating Flower Illusions

In addition to Pulsating Flower Illusions, please check out our complete list of illusions.

Pulsating Illusions

Pulsating Illusions

Check out these Pulsating Illusions that create a sense of movement or pulsation in the visual field, even when there is no actual motion occurring. These types illusions can be caused by a variety of visual stimuli, including patterns, colors, and shapes.

If you are interested in learning more about Pulsating Illusions, scroll down to read more about them. Also, check out these cool Pulsating Flower Illusions.

Pulsating Illusions
Pulsating Illusions


Table of Contents

What are Pulsating Illusions?

Pulsating Illusions that create a sense of movement or pulsation in the visual field, even when there is no actual motion occurring. These types illusions can be caused by a variety of visual stimuli, including patterns, colors, and shapes. They are usually caused by the way our brains process visual information.

Our brains constantly receive and process sensory information from our environment, including visual information from our eyes. When we look at an object or scene, our brain uses a variety of processing mechanisms to interpret the visual information and create a perception of what we are seeing.

In the case of pulsating illusions, the visual information is processed in a way that creates a rhythmic, pulsing effect. This can happen for a variety of reasons, including the way that certain visual elements in the illusion interact with each other, or the way that our brain processes and integrates different visual inputs.

For example, one common type of pulsating illusion is the “grid illusion,” where a grid of intersecting lines appears to pulsate and move. This effect is thought to be caused by the way that the intersections between the lines create contrasting areas of light and dark, which our brain interprets as moving or pulsing.

Overall, these illusions are a fascinating example of how our brains process visual information and can be used to study the mechanisms of perception and cognition.

How do Pulsating Illusions Work?

Pulsating illusions work by creating a sense of movement or pulsation in the visual field, even when there is no actual motion occurring. These illusions can be caused by a variety of visual stimuli, including patterns, colors, and shapes.

One common example of a pulsating illusion is the “Neon Color Spreading” illusion, where a brightly colored object appears to pulsate and spread color to its surroundings. This illusion is created by the way that our brain processes visual information about color and brightness. When two contrasting colors, such as red and green, are placed next to each other, they create a visual contrast that our brain interprets as pulsing or moving.

Another example is the “Grid Illusion,” where a grid of intersecting lines appears to pulsate and move. This illusion is caused by the way that the intersections between the lines create contrasting areas of light and dark, which our brain interprets as moving or pulsing.

In general, pulsating illusions are thought to be caused by the way that our brain processes and integrates visual information. By studying these illusions, scientists can gain insights into the mechanisms of perception and cognition, and how our brains create our sense of reality.

Some Similar Illusions

There are many different types of illusions that are similar in that they create a sense of movement or distortion in the visual field. Here are a few examples:

  1. Motion aftereffect illusion: This illusion occurs when you stare at a moving image for a period of time and then look at a stationary object, which appears to move in the opposite direction. This effect is caused by the way that our brain adapts to the motion of the original image and then overcompensates when we look at the stationary object.
  2. Pinna-Brelstaff illusion: This illusion occurs when you look at a spiraling image that is also rotating. The image appears to move in the opposite direction of the rotation, creating a sense of disorientation and distortion.
  3. Ames room illusion: In this illusion, a room is constructed with one corner closer to the viewer than the other, creating the illusion of a trapezoidal shape. When people enter the room, they appear to shrink or grow in size as they move from one corner to the other. This effect is caused by the way that our brain processes visual depth and perspective.
  4. Ponzo illusion: This illusion occurs when two lines of the same length are placed in a converging pattern with two diagonal lines. The line that is closer to the converging point appears longer, even though it is actually the same length as the other line. This effect is caused by the way that our brain interprets visual cues such as perspective and depth.

Overall, illusions are a fascinating way to explore how our brain processes visual information and constructs our perception of reality.

Discovery of Pulsating Illusions

Pulsating illusions, like many types of visual illusions, have been studied and documented by a number of scientists and researchers over the years. It’s difficult to attribute the discovery or popularization of pulsating illusions to any one person or group.

One of the earliest documented examples of a pulsating illusion is the “phi phenomenon,” which was first described by the psychologist Max Wertheimer in 1912. The phi phenomenon occurs when two or more visual stimuli are presented in rapid succession, creating the illusion of movement or pulsation.

Since then, many other researchers have studied pulsating illusions and related phenomena, including the ways that our brain processes visual information and creates the perception of movement and motion. Some notable contributors to this field include the psychologists Richard Gregory, Edward Adelson, and Akiyoshi Kitaoka, among others.

Today, pulsating illusions continue to be a fascinating area of study for researchers in fields like psychology, neuroscience, and cognitive science, and new discoveries are constantly being made about the ways that our brains interpret and process visual information.


References and Resources – Pulsating Illusions

In addition to Pulsating Illusions, please check out our complete list of illusions.

Breathing Square Illusion

Breathing Square Illusion

In the Breathing Square Illusion, the blue square appears to pulsate or breath, when in reality it is just spinning while being partially occluded.

Breathing Square Illusion

The Breathing Square Illusion is simply a version of the pulsating square illusion shown below. In the pulsating version, the amount of occlusion from the yellow squares changes revealing the true size and direction of the blue circle.

pulsating square illusion

The both the Breathing square illusion and the Pulsating Square Illusion are caused by a variety of phenomenon. Two of the most important are the ones observable in the Occlusion illusion and the Motion Binding Illusion

If you are interested in learning more about how the Breathing Square Illusion works, scroll down to read more about it.


Table of Contents

What is the Breathing Square Illusion?

The Breathing Square Illusion is caused by a variety of phenomenon. Two of the most important are demonstrated by the Occlusion illusion and the Motion Binding Illusion

The occlusion illusion is an optical illusion that occurs when one object appears to pass behind another object, even though the two objects are actually at the same distance from the viewer. The illusion is created by the way that the brain processes visual information about the relative positions of objects in space. Here is an image of the Occlusion illusion:

The motion binding illusion is an optical illusion that occurs when a moving object appears to be bound to another object, even though the two objects are not physically connected or interacting with each other. The illusion is created by the way that the brain processes visual information about the motion of objects in the visual field. Here is an example of the Motion Binding Illusion:

Motion Bind Illusions
Created by Michael Bach

How does the Breathing Square Illusion?

The Breathing Square Illusion is caused by a variety of phenomenon. Two of the most important are the ones observable in the Occlusion illusion and the Motion Binding Illusion

The occlusion illusion is an optical illusion that occurs when one object appears to pass behind another object, even though the two objects are actually at the same distance from the viewer. The illusion is created by the way that the brain processes visual information about the relative positions of objects in space.

When one object partially obscures another object, the brain assumes that the partially obscured object is farther away than the object that is doing the obscuring. This is because in the natural world, objects that are farther away tend to be partially obscured by closer objects.

The occlusion illusion occurs when this assumption is incorrect, and the two objects are actually at the same distance from the viewer. In this case, the brain interprets the partially obscured object as being farther away than it actually is, which creates the illusion that it is passing behind the other object.

There are several factors that can influence the strength of the occlusion illusion, such as the size and shape of the objects, the position of the viewer, and the lighting conditions. In general, the illusion is strongest when the partially obscured object is small and located near the edge of the other object, and when the viewer is positioned in such a way that the illusion is maximized.

The occlusion illusion is a fascinating example of how the brain uses visual cues to interpret the three-dimensional world around us, and how these cues can sometimes be misleading.

The motion binding illusion is an optical illusion that occurs when a moving object appears to be bound to another object, even though the two objects are not physically connected or interacting with each other. The illusion is created by the way that the brain processes visual information about the motion of objects in the visual field.

When two objects are moving independently of each other but in close proximity, the brain may perceive them as moving together or “bound” to each other. This perception is thought to occur because the brain tends to group together objects that are similar in appearance or behavior, and to interpret them as part of the same visual entity.

The motion binding illusion can be demonstrated using a simple animation of two dots moving independently of each other. When the dots are close enough to each other, they can appear to be moving together or “connected” in some way, even though there is no physical connection between them.

One theory behind the motion binding illusion is that it is related to the way that the brain processes visual information about the relative positions and motions of objects in the visual field. When objects are moving in close proximity, the brain may interpret their motions as being related or connected in some way, even if there is no physical interaction between them.

The motion binding illusion was first described in a scientific paper published in 1998 by a team of researchers led by David Whitney at the University of California, Berkeley. The study was titled “Motion Integration Across Separated Stimuli” and was published in the journal Perception & Psychophysics.

The motion binding illusion is a fascinating example of how the brain uses visual information to create a coherent perception of the world around us, and how this perception can sometimes be influenced by subtle visual cues.

Some Similar Illusions to the Breathing Square Illusion

There are several other visual illusions that are similar to the Breathing Square Illusion in terms of their effects on the perception of motion and spatial relationships between objects. Here are a few examples:

  1. The apparent motion illusion: In this illusion, a series of still images presented in rapid succession can create the perception of continuous motion, even though each image is stationary.
  2. The phi phenomenon: This is a related illusion in which two or more stationary lights flashing in sequence can create the perception of a single light moving back and forth between them.
  3. The watercolor illusion: In this illusion, the edges of a colored region appear to be darker on one side than the other, creating the illusion of a shadow, even though there is no actual shadow present.
  4. The wagon wheel illusion: In this illusion, a spoked wheel appears to be moving backwards when it is actually rotating forwards, due to the way that the brain processes visual information about rotating objects.
  5. The motion aftereffect: This is a phenomenon in which prolonged exposure to a moving stimulus can create a temporary change in the perception of motion, such as perceiving stationary objects as moving in the opposite direction.

These illusions, like the motion binding illusion, all involve the brain’s interpretation of visual information about motion and spatial relationships between objects, and they demonstrate the complexity of visual perception.


References and Resources

In addition to the breathing squares illusion check out our complete list of illusions.

Pulsating Square Illusion

pulsating square illusion

This Pulsating Square Illusion creates the appearance that the blue square is pulsating when the yellow squares are large. But, when the yellow squares are small, you can clearly see that the blue square isn’t pulsating at all.

When the the blue square’s motion is occluded, our visual system concentrates on the most prominent feature, the movement of the edges, making it appear to pulsate.

The Pulsating Square Illusion is caused by a variety of phenomenon. Two of the most important are the ones observable in the Occlusion illusion and the Motion Binding Illusion

If you are interested in learning more about how the Pulsating Square Illusion works, scroll down to read more about it.

pulsating square illusion


Table of Contents

What is the Pulsating Square Illusion?

The Pulsating Square Illusion is caused by a variety of phenomenon. Two of the most important are the ones observable in the Occlusion illusion and the Motion Binding Illusion

The occlusion illusion is an optical illusion that occurs when one object appears to pass behind another object, even though the two objects are actually at the same distance from the viewer. The illusion is created by the way that the brain processes visual information about the relative positions of objects in space. Here is an image of the Occlusion illusion:

The motion binding illusion is an optical illusion that occurs when a moving object appears to be bound to another object, even though the two objects are not physically connected or interacting with each other. The illusion is created by the way that the brain processes visual information about the motion of objects in the visual field. Here is an example of the Motion Binding Illusion:

Motion Bind Illusions
Created by Michael Bach

How does the Pulsating Square Illusion?

The Pulsating Square Illusion is caused by a variety of phenomenon. Two of the most important are the ones observable in the Occlusion illusion and the Motion Binding Illusion

The occlusion illusion is an optical illusion that occurs when one object appears to pass behind another object, even though the two objects are actually at the same distance from the viewer. The illusion is created by the way that the brain processes visual information about the relative positions of objects in space.

When one object partially obscures another object, the brain assumes that the partially obscured object is farther away than the object that is doing the obscuring. This is because in the natural world, objects that are farther away tend to be partially obscured by closer objects.

The occlusion illusion occurs when this assumption is incorrect, and the two objects are actually at the same distance from the viewer. In this case, the brain interprets the partially obscured object as being farther away than it actually is, which creates the illusion that it is passing behind the other object.

There are several factors that can influence the strength of the occlusion illusion, such as the size and shape of the objects, the position of the viewer, and the lighting conditions. In general, the illusion is strongest when the partially obscured object is small and located near the edge of the other object, and when the viewer is positioned in such a way that the illusion is maximized.

The occlusion illusion is a fascinating example of how the brain uses visual cues to interpret the three-dimensional world around us, and how these cues can sometimes be misleading.

The motion binding illusion is an optical illusion that occurs when a moving object appears to be bound to another object, even though the two objects are not physically connected or interacting with each other. The illusion is created by the way that the brain processes visual information about the motion of objects in the visual field.

When two objects are moving independently of each other but in close proximity, the brain may perceive them as moving together or “bound” to each other. This perception is thought to occur because the brain tends to group together objects that are similar in appearance or behavior, and to interpret them as part of the same visual entity.

The motion binding illusion can be demonstrated using a simple animation of two dots moving independently of each other. When the dots are close enough to each other, they can appear to be moving together or “connected” in some way, even though there is no physical connection between them.

One theory behind the motion binding illusion is that it is related to the way that the brain processes visual information about the relative positions and motions of objects in the visual field. When objects are moving in close proximity, the brain may interpret their motions as being related or connected in some way, even if there is no physical interaction between them.

The motion binding illusion was first described in a scientific paper published in 1998 by a team of researchers led by David Whitney at the University of California, Berkeley. The study was titled “Motion Integration Across Separated Stimuli” and was published in the journal Perception & Psychophysics.

The motion binding illusion is a fascinating example of how the brain uses visual information to create a coherent perception of the world around us, and how this perception can sometimes be influenced by subtle visual cues.

Some Similar Illusions to the Pulsating Square Illusion

There are several other visual illusions that are similar to the Pulsating Square Illusion in terms of their effects on the perception of motion and spatial relationships between objects. Here are a few examples:

  1. The apparent motion illusion: In this illusion, a series of still images presented in rapid succession can create the perception of continuous motion, even though each image is stationary.
  2. The phi phenomenon: This is a related illusion in which two or more stationary lights flashing in sequence can create the perception of a single light moving back and forth between them.
  3. The watercolor illusion: In this illusion, the edges of a colored region appear to be darker on one side than the other, creating the illusion of a shadow, even though there is no actual shadow present.
  4. The wagon wheel illusion: In this illusion, a spoked wheel appears to be moving backwards when it is actually rotating forwards, due to the way that the brain processes visual information about rotating objects.
  5. The motion aftereffect: This is a phenomenon in which prolonged exposure to a moving stimulus can create a temporary change in the perception of motion, such as perceiving stationary objects as moving in the opposite direction.

These illusions, like the motion binding illusion, all involve the brain’s interpretation of visual information about motion and spatial relationships between objects, and they demonstrate the complexity of visual perception.


References and Resources

In addition to the pulsating squares illusion check out our complete list of illusions.

Perfect Squares Illusion

Perfect Squares Illusion

In this Perfect Squares Illusion every one of the red boxes is a perfect square.

To see for yourself, try moving away from the screen so that the black and white lines begin to fade away. As they fade away, you should be able to see the red squares more accurately, as perfectly square.

This illusion is being created by the combination of two famous illusions – the Zöllner Illusion and the Poggendorff Illusion.

Perfect Squares Illusion
Artist Pearl Whitecrow Brown


Table of Contents

What is the Perfect Squares Illusion?

The Perfect Squares Illusion is being created by the combination of two famous illusions – the Zöllner Illusion and the Poggendorff Illusion.

The Zöllner illusion is an optical illusion that involves a series of parallel lines intersected by diagonal lines. The diagonal lines can appear to be either tilted or straight, depending on their orientation and the orientation of the surrounding lines. The illusion was first described by Johann Karl Friedrich Zöllner in 1860. Here is an image of the Zöllner illusion

Zöllner illusion

The Poggendorff illusion is an optical illusion that involves the misperception of the position of a diagonal line that is interrupted by a rectangular object. The illusion was first described by the German physicist Johann Poggendorff in 1860. Here is a picture of the Poggendorff illusion.

Poppendorff Illusion Lines

How does the Perfect Squares Illusion Work?

The Perfect Squares Illusion is being created by the combination of two famous illusions – the Zöllner Illusion and the Poggendorff Illusion.

The Zöllner illusion occurs because the brain tries to interpret the lines as either parallel or tilted, but the diagonal lines disrupt this interpretation. As a result, the brain can perceive the diagonal lines as being tilted in the opposite direction to their actual orientation.

The Zöllner illusion is an example of a geometric-optical illusion, which means that it involves the interaction of geometric patterns with the visual system. This type of illusion occurs because the brain has to interpret the two-dimensional image presented to it as a three-dimensional object in order to make sense of it. In the case of the Zöllner illusion, the diagonal lines can be interpreted as either being on top of or underneath the parallel lines, which can lead to conflicting interpretations of the orientation of the diagonal lines.

The Poggendorff illusion is an optical illusion that involves the misperception of the position of a diagonal line that is interrupted by a rectangular object. The illusion was first described by the German physicist Johann Poggendorff in 1860.

In the classic version of the Poggendorff illusion, a diagonal line is interrupted by a rectangular object that is positioned at an angle to the line. The line appears to be displaced, and the angle at which it crosses the rectangle appears to be different from its true angle.

The exact mechanism behind the Poggendorff illusion is still a matter of debate, but it is thought to involve the brain’s processing of visual information about the angles and positions of objects in space. One theory is that the brain tends to group objects that are aligned in a certain way, and to perceive them as forming a continuous line or curve. When the diagonal line is interrupted by the rectangle, the brain tries to create a continuous path for the line, but this can lead to a misperception of its position and angle.

Another theory is that the Poggendorff illusion is related to the brain’s processing of depth and perspective cues. When the diagonal line is interrupted by the rectangle, the brain may perceive the line as being positioned at a different depth than it actually is, which can lead to a misperception of its position and angle.

Overall, the Poggendorff illusion is a classic example of how the brain can be tricked by visual information, and it highlights the complex processes involved in perceiving the visual world.

Some Similar Illusions to the Perfect Squares Illusion

There are several other visual illusions that are similar to the Perfect Squares Illusion in terms of their effects on the perception of lines and angles. Here are a few examples:

  1. The Hering illusion: In this illusion, two parallel lines appear to be bowed outwards when they are intersected by two diagonal lines that are oriented in opposite directions.
  2. The Müller-Lyer illusion: In this illusion, two lines of equal length appear to be different lengths due to the presence of arrow-like lines on either end of the lines.
  3. The Ponzo illusion: In this illusion, two identical lines appear to be different lengths when they are placed in a converging perspective drawing that includes parallel lines that create the illusion of depth.
  4. The Ebbinghaus illusion: In this illusion, a circle appears to be larger or smaller depending on the size of the surrounding circles, even though the central circle is actually the same size in both cases.

These illusions, like the Perfect Squares Illusion, all involve the brain’s interpretation of visual information about lines, angles, and shapes, and they demonstrate the complexity of visual perception.


References and Resources

Check out our complete list of illusions.