Crater Illusion

Crater Illusion

The crater illusion is a visual illusion that make it seem that craters appear to be raised, when if fact, they are indented.

Crater Illusion
The Crater Illusion

Table of Contents


How does the Crater Illusion work?

The illusion is created by the brain’s interpretation of depth and perspective. The circles appear to be raised or indented because the brain interprets the circles that are farther away as smaller and those that are closer as larger. This creates the illusion of depth and the appearance of a series of shallow or deep depressions.

The illusion is a variation of the well-known “Ebbinghaus illusion” which is based on the same principle of relative size perception, but it’s composed by circles instead of circles and circles and it’s called the “crater illusion” because the circles resemble the shape of craters.

It is important to note that the illusion is purely visual, the circles themselves are actually the same size, and there is no actual depth or depression.

Versions of the Crater Illusion

The following is another example of the Crater Illusion:


Crater Illusion

Illusions like the Crater Illusion

The crater illusion is a type of visual illusion known as a size-distance illusion. Size-distance illusions involve the perception of objects that appear to be different sizes based on their distance from the viewer. The crater illusion specifically is a variation of the well-known “Ebbinghaus illusion” which is a size-distance illusion that occurs when two circles of the same size are surrounded by circles of different sizes.

Size-distance illusions are caused by the way our brains perceive and interpret visual information. Our brains use various cues, such as relative size, texture, and lighting to judge the distance of objects in our environment. In the case of the crater illusion, the brain interprets the circles that are farther away as smaller and those that are closer as larger, which creates the illusion of depth and the appearance of a series of shallow or deep depressions.

It’s important to note that the illusion is purely visual, the circles themselves are actually the same size, and there is no actual depth or depression.

The Ebbinghaus illusion (also known as Titchener circles) is a perceptual illusion in which the perceived size of a central circle is affected by the size of the surrounding circles. The central circle appears smaller when surrounded by larger circles, and larger when surrounded by smaller circles.

Ebbinghaus Illusion


The Delboeuf illusion is similar to the Ebbinghaus illusion, but instead of circles, it uses two concentric circles or rings. The central ring appears larger or smaller depending on the size of the surrounding ring.

Delboeuf Illusion
delboeuf illusion with food.

The Poggendorff illusion is another size-contrast illusion in which a straight line appears to be bent or angled, due to the presence of intersecting lines or shapes in the background.

Poppendorff Illusion Lines

The Zöllner illusion is another size-contrast illusion in which a group of diagonal lines appear to be distorted or bent due to the presence of intersecting lines.

Zöllner illusion


The Müller-Lyer illusion is a classic example of size-contrast illusion, in which two lines of equal length appear to be different due to the presence of arrowheads or fins at the end of the lines.

müller-lyer illusion

All of these illusions rely on the way the visual system processes relative size information, and they all highlight the complexity and malleability of visual perception.


Discovery of the Crater Illusion

The origins of the crater illusion are not well-documented, but it is considered a variation of the well-known “Ebbinghaus illusion” which was first described by the German psychologist Hermann Ebbinghaus in his 1885 book “Über das Gedächtnis” (On Memory).

He observed that when two circles of the same size are surrounded by circles of different sizes, the central circles appear to be different sizes. This illusion is an example of the size-distance illusion, which occurs when the perception of the size of an object is affected by its distance from the viewer.

It’s important to note that the crater illusion, as a specific variation of the Ebbinghaus illusion, is a well-established phenomenon in the field of psychology and it’s widely studied by researchers in the field of visual perception.

The Ebbinghaus illusion is named after German psychologist Hermann Ebbinghaus, who first described it in the late 19th century. The illusion is thought to be caused by the way the brain processes relative size information in visual scenes.

Hermann Ebbinghaus (1850-1909) was a German psychologist who is best known for his pioneering research on memory and forgetting. He was one of the first researchers to systematically study memory as a psychological process and his work laid the foundation for the field of experimental psychology. Ebbinghaus conducted a series of experiments in which he memorized lists of nonsense syllables and then tested his own memory at various intervals to measure the rate of forgetting.

He also developed the first standardized memory test, called the “memory drum”, which consisted of a rotating drum with lists of words or syllables that could be presented to participants at different intervals. He used these tests to study the effects of different factors, such as repetition and interference, on memory retention.

Ebbinghaus also made significant contributions to the understanding of memory processes, such as the spacing effect, which states that spaced practice is more effective than massed practice in enhancing memory retention.

Ebbinghaus’s work was groundbreaking in the field of psychology and is still widely cited today. The Ebbinghaus illusion, the size-contrast illusion that bears his name, was discovered by him in 1885 but is not as well-known as his memory research.

References and Resources

Check out our complete list of illusions.

Benham’s Top

The Benham’s top illusion is the visual effect created when the top is spun, in which the pattern appears to change colors.

The illusion is caused by the phenomenon of persistence of vision, in which the brain continues to perceive an image for a short period of time after it has disappeared.

As the top spins, the brain perceives the pattern as if it were changing colors due to the persistence of the previous image. This is an optical illusion and the color does not actually change.

Benham's Top
Benham’s Top


Table of Contents


How does Benham’s Top work?

The Benham’s top illusion works by exploiting the phenomenon of persistence of vision. Persistence of vision is a characteristic of the human visual system where an image that is presented for a short period of time is perceived to persist for a fraction of a second longer.

When the Benham’s top is spun, the pattern on the top moves quickly past the viewer’s eyes, and because of persistence of vision, the brain perceives the pattern as if it were changing colors. The pattern on the top is composed of alternating bright and dark colors, and as the top spins, the brain perceives the dark colors as one color and the bright colors as another color, creating the illusion that the top is changing colors.

Additionally, the speed of rotation and the design of the pattern are important factors that affect the illusion. A faster spinning top will make the illusion more pronounced, and a pattern with more distinct colors will be more effective at creating the illusion.

Benham’s top and Fechner color are two different phenomena, but they are related and based on the same principle of persistence of vision.

Benham’s top is a mechanical device that uses a spinning disc with a pattern of alternating bright and dark colors to create the illusion of the disc changing color as it spins. This illusion is created by the brain’s persistence of vision, which makes it perceive the dark and bright colors as different colors.

Fechner color, named after the German psychologist Gustav Fechner, is a similar visual illusion that occurs when a rotating ring with a pattern of alternating colors is viewed. Like Benham’s top, the illusion is created by the brain’s persistence of vision, which makes the viewer perceive the ring as if the colors were blending together.

Both Benham’s top and Fechner color are examples of the persistence of vision phenomenon and how it can create optical illusions.

Versions of the Benham’s Top

The first is the image when still, the second is when it’s spinning:



Benham's Top





Illusions like Benham’s Top

Some related illusions include the following:

A color constancy illusion is a type of visual illusion in which a color appears to be different when viewed in different contexts.

The Checker Shadow Illusion is created by a checkerboard pattern composed of squares with different luminance values, the squares that are not directly illuminated by the light source appear darker than the illuminated squares, creating the illusion of shadows.

Edelson-Checker_shadow_illusion
Checker Shadow Illusion


The simultaneous contrast illusion is a visual effect that occurs when the perception of a color is affected by the colors of the surrounding area.

The illusion creates the appearance of a change in the color of an object, even though the actual color of the object remains constant.


Simultaneous Contrast Effect
The center green dot is the same on both sides, but the surrounding color changes the perception


The Neon Color Spreading illusion refers to the visual phenomenon where an area of color appears to spread or “bleed” beyond its intended boundaries.

Neon Color Spreading
From Wikimedia Commons


The Bezold Effect: This illusion is created by placing two or more colors next to each other, and the way they appear to change when they are close to one another.  

Bezold_Effect
from Wikimedia Commons


The Cornsweet illusion is a classic example of a brightness illusion, which is an illusion in which two areas that are physically the same brightness appear to be different in brightness.

Cornsweet illusion
Cornsweet illusion

The Chubb illusion is based on the perception of brightness and can be observed when a small bright patch is surrounded by a larger dark area, the small bright patch will appear brighter than the same patch surrounded by a bright area.

Chubb Illusion

White’s illusion is a visual phenomenon in which two identical gray bars are placed on a background of alternating black and white stripes.

The gray bars appear to be different shades of gray, with the one on the white stripes appearing lighter than the one on the black stripes.

In the image below, both gray bars have the exact same color.

White's Illusion
White’s Illusion

The Watercolor Illusion: This illusion is created by the way the brain perceives edges of an object. When an object is surrounded by a colored halo, the object appears to have a different color than it actually does.

Watercolor Illusion


The Café Wall Illusion is a visual illusion that is created by a grid of alternating light and dark horizontal and vertical lines. The lines appear to be bent or tilted, even though they are actually straight.

Cafe Wall Illusion


Discovery of Benham’s Top

Benham’s top was discovered by S.G. Benham, a British scientist and inventor in the late 19th century.

He created the top as an optical toy that demonstrated the phenomenon of persistence of vision, and its ability to create the illusion of color change.

He published his work on the top in 1897 in the scientific journal “Nature“. His invention was popularized as a toy and used in demonstrations of optical phenomena in science education and entertainment shows.

References and Resources

Check out our complete list of illusions.

Sign Up to Have Your Mind Blown

Color Constancy

Color constancy

A color constancy illusion is a type of visual illusion in which a color appears to be different when viewed in different contexts.

For example, the same patch of color may appear lighter or darker when viewed against different backgrounds, or may appear to change color when viewed under different lighting conditions.

These illusions are thought to be caused by the way the human visual system processes color information, and can have important implications for fields such as graphic design and color science.

Color constancy
Color Constancy


Table of Contents


How does the Color Constancy work?

Color constancy illusions are thought to be caused by the way the human visual system processes color information. The human eye and brain work together to perceive color, and the perception of color is influenced by a variety of factors including the surrounding colors, lighting conditions, and the physical characteristics of the object being viewed.

One theory is that these illusions occur because the human visual system uses surrounding colors to interpret the color of an object. When the surrounding colors are different, the same object can appear to have a different color. This is known as the “Simultaneous Contrast” illusion.

Another theory is that the human visual system uses the lighting conditions to interpret the color of an object, and this can lead to illusions. For example, an object may appear to be one color in natural daylight and another color under artificial light. This is known as the “Metamerism” illusion.

Additionally, the physical characteristics of the object being viewed can also influence the perception of color. For example, the texture, surface finish, and gloss of an object can all affect how it reflects light and how it appears to the observer. This is known as the “Interference” illusion.

Overall, color constancy illusions are the result of the complex interaction between the human visual system and the physical and environmental factors that influence the perception of color.

Versions of the Color Constancy

The following are other examples of the Color Constancy:



Color constancy



Color constancy



Illusions like Color Constancy

Some related illusions include the following:

The Checker Shadow Illusion is created by a checkerboard pattern composed of squares with different luminance values, the squares that are not directly illuminated by the light source appear darker than the illuminated squares, creating the illusion of shadows.

Edelson-Checker_shadow_illusion
Checker Shadow Illusion


The simultaneous contrast illusion is a visual effect that occurs when the perception of a color is affected by the colors of the surrounding area.

The illusion creates the appearance of a change in the color of an object, even though the actual color of the object remains constant.


Simultaneous Contrast Effect
The center green dot is the same on both sides, but the surrounding color changes the perception


The Neon Color Spreading illusion refers to the visual phenomenon where an area of color appears to spread or “bleed” beyond its intended boundaries.

Neon Color Spreading
From Wikimedia Commons


The Bezold Effect: This illusion is created by placing two or more colors next to each other, and the way they appear to change when they are close to one another.  

Bezold_Effect
from Wikimedia Commons


The Cornsweet illusion is a classic example of a brightness illusion, which is an illusion in which two areas that are physically the same brightness appear to be different in brightness.

Cornsweet illusion
Cornsweet illusion

The Chubb illusion is based on the perception of brightness and can be observed when a small bright patch is surrounded by a larger dark area, the small bright patch will appear brighter than the same patch surrounded by a bright area.

Chubb Illusion

White’s illusion is a visual phenomenon in which two identical gray bars are placed on a background of alternating black and white stripes.

The gray bars appear to be different shades of gray, with the one on the white stripes appearing lighter than the one on the black stripes.

In the image below, both gray bars have the exact same color.

White's Illusion
White’s Illusion

The Watercolor Illusion: This illusion is created by the way the brain perceives edges of an object. When an object is surrounded by a colored halo, the object appears to have a different color than it actually does.

Watercolor Illusion


The Café Wall Illusion is a visual illusion that is created by a grid of alternating light and dark horizontal and vertical lines. The lines appear to be bent or tilted, even though they are actually straight.

Cafe Wall Illusion


Discovery of the Color Constancy

The concept of color constancy, which refers to the ability of the human visual system to perceive the colors of objects as constant despite changes in the lighting conditions, was first described by the German physiologist Ewald Hering in the late 19th century.

Hering proposed the opponent-process theory of color vision, which posits that the human visual system processes color information through three pairs of opponent channels: red-green, blue-yellow, and black-white. According to this theory, the visual system uses the relative activity of these opponent channels to perceive color, which allows it to maintain color constancy despite changes in the lighting conditions.

Hering’s theory of color vision and color constancy has been influential in the field of color science and continues to be studied by researchers in the fields of psychology, neuroscience, and computer vision.

wald Hering was a German physiologist and psychologist who lived from 1834 to 1918. He is best known for his work on the physiology of the senses and the physiology of the nervous system. He was one of the first to study the physiology of the senses in a systematic manner, and he made important contributions to the understanding of color vision and color constancy.

Hering was a professor of physiology at the University of Prague and later at the University of Würzburg. He was a leading figure in the field of physiology in the late 19th century, and his work on the physiology of the senses was widely recognized and influential.

One of his key contributions was his opponent-process theory of color vision, which he proposed in 1878. This theory posits that the human visual system processes color information through three pairs of opponent channels: red-green, blue-yellow, and black-white. This theory has been very influential in the field of color science and is still widely accepted today.

In addition to his contributions to color science, Hering also made important contributions to the field of psychology, particularly in the areas of perception, memory, and attention.

References and Resources

Check out our complete list of illusions.

Sign Up to Have Your Mind Blown

Ames Window

Ames Window

The Ames Window (or trapezoid) is a flat object that seems to be a rectangular window but is actually a trapezoid.

The illusion is created by manipulating the perspective through the use of angles.

The image below shows the Ames Window, but scroll down to the alternate versions and the video to see this amazing illusion in action.

Ames Window
Ames Window


Table of Contents


How does the Ames Window Illusion work?

The Ames Window is a type of illusion known as a perspective illusion, which is a visual illusion that occurs when the perceived size, shape, or position of an object is different from its actual size, shape, or position, due to the way that the eyes and brain process visual information.

Perspective illusions are created by manipulating the visual cues that the brain uses to perceive depth, such as the relative size and position of objects, the angle of the lines and edges, and the amount of light and shadow.

The Ames Window is a specific type of perspective illusion that uses the manipulation of perspective to create the illusion.


Versions of the Ames Window Illusion

The following are some alternate versions of the Ames Window Illusion:



Ames Window



Illusions like the Ames Window Illusion

There are many other visual perception illusions that work in similar ways to the Ames Window illusion. Some examples include:

The Ames Room illusion is a visual illusion in which a room appears distorted, causing people or objects within it to appear differently sized.

Ames Room Black and White
Ames Room

The Müller-Lyer illusion is a visual perception illusion that consists of two lines of equal length, with arrows at each end pointing either inward or outward. The line with the inward-pointing arrows appears shorter than the line with the outward-pointing arrows, even though they are actually the same length.

müller-lyer illusion
The Müller-Lyer Illusion

The Poggendorff illusion: This illusion is similar to the Müller-Lyer illusion, but instead of arrowheads, it uses a diagonal line that intersects two parallel lines. The brain perceives the diagonal line as being tilted, causing the parallel lines to appear as if they are not parallel.

Poppendorff Illusion Lines


The Ebbinghaus illusion: This illusion involves circles of different sizes, but with the same-sized circles placed close to each other. The brain perceives the smaller circles as being larger when they are placed next to larger circles.

Ebbinghaus Illusion


The T illusion: This illusion involves a T-shaped figure with a horizontal bar and a vertical bar. The brain perceives the vertical bar as being longer than the horizontal bar, even though they are the same length.

T Illusion


The Kanizsa triangle: This illusion is created by placing Pac-Man-like shapes around an invisible triangle. The brain perceives the triangle as if it were a real, visible triangle.

Kanizsa Triangle Illusion


The Zöllner illusion: This illusion involves parallel lines with diagonal lines intersecting them. The brain perceives the lines as if they were not parallel and that the diagonal lines are converging.

Zöllner illusion


Moon illusion: This illusion involves the perception of the Moon appearing larger when it is near the horizon compared to when it is high in the sky.

Moon Illusion


Ponzo illusion: This illusion involves the perception of an object appearing larger or smaller depending on the size of the surrounding context.

Ponzo Illusion


The Café Wall Illusion is a visual illusion that is created by a grid of alternating light and dark horizontal and vertical lines. The lines appear to be bent or tilted, even though they are actually straight.

Cafe Wall Illusion


All these illusions are based on the brain’s perception of visual context, depth cues, and grouping rules. These illusions help to understand how the brain interprets visual information and how it can be deceived.

Discovery of the Ames Window Illusion

The illusion was first described by American psychologist Adelbert Ames Jr. in 1947, and is often used in research on visual perception and optical illusions.

Ames was a professor of psychology at the University of Michigan and later at Harvard University.

He was known for his research on visual perception, optical illusions, and the role of the visual system in the perception of motion.

Ames was also known for his work on the psychology of perception in relation to art and design.

He published several papers and articles on the Ames Room illusion and its implications for understanding the visual system.

References and Resources

Check out our complete list of illusions.


Sign Up to Have Your Mind Blown

Ames Room

Ames Room Black and White

The Ames Room illusion is a visual illusion in which a room appears distorted, causing people or objects within it to appear differently sized.

The illusion is created by manipulating the perspective of the room through the use of angled walls and a skewed ceiling, which causes the viewer’s perception of depth and size to be altered.

Ames Room Black and White
Ames Room


Table of Contents


How does the Ames Room Illusion work?

The Ames Room illusion works by manipulating the perspective of the room in such a way that the viewer’s perception of depth and size is altered.

This is achieved by building the room with angled walls and a skewed ceiling, which causes the room to appear distorted.

The illusion is created by making one corner of the room appear closer to the viewer than the other, while keeping the size of the room constant.

This causes the objects or people within the room to appear differently sized, as the closer corner appears larger and the farther corner appears smaller.

The illusion is enhanced by placing a person or object in each corner and having them move, it will appear as if one is bigger than the other, even though they are the same size.

The illusion is so convincing that it can be difficult to believe that the room is not actually distorted, and it is often used in research on visual perception and optical illusions.


Versions of the Ames Room Illusion

The following are some alternate versions of the Ames Room Illusion:

Ames Room Image


Ames Room Diagram


Ames Room Diagram



Illusions like the Ames Room Illusion

he Ames Room is a type of illusion known as a perspective illusion, which is a visual illusion that occurs when the perceived size, shape, or position of an object is different from its actual size, shape, or position, due to the way that the eyes and brain process visual information.

Perspective illusions are created by manipulating the visual cues that the brain uses to perceive depth, such as the relative size and position of objects, the angle of the lines and edges, and the amount of light and shadow.

The Ames Room is a specific type of perspective illusion that uses the manipulation of the room’s perspective to create the illusion of differently sized people or objects within it.

The illusion is so convincing that it can be difficult to believe that the room is not actually distorted.

There are many other visual perception illusions that work in similar ways to the Ames Room illusion. Some examples include:

The Müller-Lyer illusion is a visual perception illusion that consists of two lines of equal length, with arrows at each end pointing either inward or outward. The line with the inward-pointing arrows appears shorter than the line with the outward-pointing arrows, even though they are actually the same length.

müller-lyer illusion
The Müller-Lyer Illusion

The Poggendorff illusion: This illusion is similar to the Müller-Lyer illusion, but instead of arrowheads, it uses a diagonal line that intersects two parallel lines. The brain perceives the diagonal line as being tilted, causing the parallel lines to appear as if they are not parallel.

Poppendorff Illusion Lines


The Ebbinghaus illusion: This illusion involves circles of different sizes, but with the same-sized circles placed close to each other. The brain perceives the smaller circles as being larger when they are placed next to larger circles.

Ebbinghaus Illusion


The T illusion: This illusion involves a T-shaped figure with a horizontal bar and a vertical bar. The brain perceives the vertical bar as being longer than the horizontal bar, even though they are the same length.

T Illusion


The Kanizsa triangle: This illusion is created by placing Pac-Man-like shapes around an invisible triangle. The brain perceives the triangle as if it were a real, visible triangle.

Kanizsa Triangle Illusion


The Zöllner illusion: This illusion involves parallel lines with diagonal lines intersecting them. The brain perceives the lines as if they were not parallel and that the diagonal lines are converging.

Zöllner illusion


Moon illusion: This illusion involves the perception of the Moon appearing larger when it is near the horizon compared to when it is high in the sky.

Moon Illusion


Ponzo illusion: This illusion involves the perception of an object appearing larger or smaller depending on the size of the surrounding context.

Ponzo Illusion


The Café Wall Illusion is a visual illusion that is created by a grid of alternating light and dark horizontal and vertical lines. The lines appear to be bent or tilted, even though they are actually straight.

Cafe Wall Illusion


All these illusions are based on the brain’s perception of visual context, depth cues, and grouping rules. These illusions help to understand how the brain interprets visual information and how it can be deceived.

Discovery of the Ames Room Illusion

The illusion was first described by American psychologist Adelbert Ames Jr. in the 1950s, and is often used in research on visual perception and optical illusions.

Ames was a professor of psychology at the University of Michigan and later at Harvard University.

He was known for his research on visual perception, optical illusions, and the role of the visual system in the perception of motion.

Ames was also known for his work on the psychology of perception in relation to art and design.

He published several papers and articles on the Ames Room illusion and its implications for understanding the visual system.

References and Resources

Check out our complete list of illusions.


Sign Up to Have Your Mind Blown

Ambigram

Ambigram

An ambigram is a typographical design that can be read as one or more words, not just in its normal orientation but also when it is rotated, flipped or viewed from a different angle. The word “ambigram” comes from the Latin “ambi-” (meaning “both”) and “gram” (meaning “letter” or “word”).

An ambigram is not considered an illusion in the traditional sense of the word, but it is a form of visual play that can be seen as a type of perceptual puzzle.

Ambigram
Ambigram of Ambigram

Table of Contents


How do Ambigrams work?

An ambigram is created by designing a word or phrase such that different letters or letterforms are used in different orientations, such that the word or phrase can be read in multiple ways. There are several types of ambigrams, such as rotational ambigrams, which can be read in different orientations, and reflective ambigrams, which can be read when reflected in a mirror.

Ambigrams have been used in various forms of art, typography, graphic design, tattoos, and even on book covers, logos, and other forms of branding. They have also been used in cryptography and secret messages, where the sender and the receiver must know the technique to read the message.

Ambigrams can be created by hand, or by using various computer programs such as Illustrator, and there are also online tools that can help you create your own ambigrams. There are also communities and websites dedicated to ambigrams and their designs, where artists and designers can share their work, and get inspiration from others.

Versions of Ambigrams

The following are a few Ambigrams

Ambigram


Ambigram


Illusions like Ambigrams

An ambigram is not considered an illusion in the traditional sense of the word, but it is a form of visual play that can be seen as a type of perceptual puzzle. An illusion is something that tricks the mind into seeing something that is not actually there, while an ambigram is a design that can be read in multiple ways, but it is not deceiving the viewer into seeing something that is not real.

Ambigrams are therefore most similar to Ambiguous illusions.

Ambiguous illusions are visual stimuli that can be interpreted in more than one way. These illusions typically consist of a single image that can be perceived in multiple ways. Ambiguous illusions can be used to study the neural mechanisms of perception and how the brain resolves ambiguity.

Some related illusions include the following:

The Rubin vase, also known as the Rubin face or the figure-ground vase, is a famous optical illusion in which the image of a vase can also be perceived as two faces in profile looking at each other.

Rubin Vase Classic Black and White


The duck-rabbit illusion is an optical illusion that is an image that can be perceived as either a duck or a rabbit depending on how the viewer looks at it.

Duck Rabbit Illusion

The Necker cube is an optical illusion that features a simple wireframe drawing of a cube. The cube appears to switch back and forth between two different orientations.

Necker Cube

The My Wife and My Mother-in-Law illusion is an optical illusion with an image of a young woman’s face that can also be perceived as an older woman’s face.

Wife and Mother In Law

The Schröder Staircase is an optical illusion that features a drawing of a staircase. The staircase appears to be either ascending or descending, depending on how the brain interprets the angles of the lines.

Schroeders_stairs
From Wikimedia Commons

The impossible cube is an optical illusion that depicts a three-dimensional object that is physically impossible to construct.

Impossible Cube Illusion


The Penrose triangle, also known as the Penrose tribar, is an optical illusion that depicts a three-dimensional object that is physically impossible to construct.

Penrose Triangle


The impossible trident is a three-pronged impossible shape resembling a trident. It is usually depicted as a three-pronged fork with each prong appearing to be a continuation of the next, creating an impossible shape.

Impossible Trident
From Wikimedia Commons

The spinning dancer illusion is a visual illusion that depicts a silhouette of a dancer spinning clockwise or counterclockwise. The direction of the dancer’s spin can appear to change depending on the viewer’s perception

Spinning Dancer Gif
From Wikimedia Commons

The Monroe-Einstein illusion works by playing with the viewer’s expectations about the relationships between the features of the faces and the viewer’s sense of facial proportions.

Monroe-Einstein

Discovery of Ambigrams

The origins of ambigrams are not entirely clear, but the concept has been around for centuries in various forms. The earliest known examples of ambigrams can be found in ancient art and architecture, where words or phrases were designed to be read in multiple ways. For example, many ancient cultures used palindromes, words or phrases that can be read forward and backward, in their art and writing.

In modern times, the term “ambigram” was first coined by artist and graphic designer John Langdon in the late 1970s and early 1980s. Langdon was one of the first to explore the concept of creating typographical designs that could be read in multiple ways and he is considered as one of the pioneers of modern ambigram design. He has created many ambigrams and also wrote several books on the subject.

It’s also worth mentioning that, before Langdon, the term “ambigram” was used in a mathematical context, refer to a geometric pattern or design that can be seen as the same when rotated by a specific angle, but the concept of Ambigram as a typographical design is credited to Langdon.

References and Resources

Check out our complete list of illusions.

Sign Up to Have Your Mind Blown

Ambiguous Cylinder Illusion

ambiguous-cylinder-illusion

The Ambiguous Cylinder illusion is a visual illusion that involves the perception of depth and shape in a two-dimensional image.

The illusion is created by an image of a cylinder that is viewed from an angle, which can appear to be either a cylinder or two faces in front of a cylinder.

The image is said to be “ambiguous” because it can be perceived in two different ways.

ambiguous-cylinder-illusion
Ambiguous Cylinder

Table of Contents

How does the Ambiguous Cylinder Illusion work?

The illusion is created by the way the brain interprets the shape and shading of the image. The brain uses cues such as the relative size and position of the different parts of the image, as well as the way light falls on the object, to perceive depth and shape.

In the case of the Ambiguous Cylinder illusion, the brain is presented with conflicting cues, which leads to the perception of two different interpretations of the same image.

The Ambiguous Cylinder illusion is a type of ambiguous illusion, which is a type of visual illusion in which an image can be perceived in multiple ways.

Ambiguous illusions are often created by using geometric shapes and shading to create conflicting cues about the depth and shape of an object.

In the case of the Ambiguous Cylinder illusion, the image of a cylinder viewed from an angle can be perceived as either a cylinder or two faces in front of a cylinder.

The image is said to be “ambiguous” because it can be perceived in two different ways.

It is also a kind of optical illusion, which are visual effects that are caused by the way the eye and brain process visual information. These illusions can occur because of the way the visual system interprets light, color, and motion, and can be categorized as static or kinetic illusions. The Ambiguous Cylinder illusion is a static illusion, which means that it does not involve the perception of motion.

Version of the Ambiguous Cylinder Illusion

To better understand the Ambiguous Cylinder Illusion, you should really watch this brief video:

Illusions like the Ambiguous Cylinder Illusion

The Ambiguous Cylinder illusion is an example of a “depth-ambiguous” image which can be perceived in different ways.

This illusion is interesting because it shows that our perception of depth and shape is not always based on a single, objective interpretation of the visual information.

The way the brain processes the visual information is influenced by the context and the prior knowledge.

Some related illusions include the following:

The Necker cube is an optical illusion that features a simple wireframe drawing of a cube. The cube appears to switch back and forth between two different orientations.

Necker Cube

The Schröder Staircase is an optical illusion that features a drawing of a staircase. The staircase appears to be either ascending or descending, depending on how the brain interprets the angles of the lines.

Schroeders_stairs
From Wikimedia Commons

The impossible cube is an optical illusion that depicts a three-dimensional object that is physically impossible to construct.

Impossible Cube Illusion


The Penrose triangle, also known as the Penrose tribar, is an optical illusion that depicts a three-dimensional object that is physically impossible to construct.

Penrose Triangle


The impossible trident is a three-pronged impossible shape resembling a trident. It is usually depicted as a three-pronged fork with each prong appearing to be a continuation of the next, creating an impossible shape.

Impossible Trident

The spinning dancer illusion is a visual illusion that depicts a silhouette of a dancer spinning clockwise or counterclockwise. The direction of the dancer’s spin can appear to change depending on the viewer’s perception

Spinning Dancer Gif
From Wikimedia Commons

Discovery of the Ambiguous Cylinder Illusion

A popular version of the Ambiguous Object Illusion or the Ambiguous Cylinder Illusion was designed by Kokichi Sugihara of Meiji University in Japan.

Kokichi Sugihara is a professor at Meiji University in Japan and a renowned researcher in the field of visual perception and geometric illusions. He is known for his work on the perception of three-dimensional shapes and the creation of impossible objects and other optical illusions.

Sugihara is particularly known for his work on impossible objects, which are three-dimensional shapes that appear to be impossible to construct in the real world. He has created a wide range of impossible objects and geometric illusions, many of which have been featured in exhibitions and popular science publications.

He has published many papers, articles, and books about his research on visual perception and geometric illusions. He has received several awards for his work, including the Best Illusion of the Year contest in 2010, and the The Japan Society for the Promotion of Science Prize in 2016.

Sugihara is also known for his work on the application of geometric illusions to design and engineering, he has created a wide range of impossible objects and geometric illusions, many of which have been featured in exhibitions and popular science publications.

References and Resources

Check out our complete list of illusions.

Sign Up to Have Your Mind Blown

The Pinna Illusion

Pinna Original

The Pinna Illusion is created by displaying a pattern of light and dark bars on a computer screen. The pattern appears to rotate, even though it is actually stationary.

The Pinna Illusion is similar to the Pinna-Brelstaff illusion, but it is created by the interaction between light and dark regions of the image, rather than bars.

Pinna Original
Pinna Illusion


Table of Contents

How does the Pinna Illusion work?

The Pinna illusion is a visual illusion that was discovered by Bruno Pinna, a cognitive scientist and researcher in the field of visual perception. The illusion is created by displaying a pattern of light and dark regions on a computer screen. The pattern appears to rotate, even though it is actually stationary.

The illusion works by exploiting the way our visual system processes information about motion. The brain uses a variety of cues, such as the relative motion of different parts of an image, to perceive motion. The Pinna illusion takes advantage of these cues by presenting an image that tricks the brain into perceiving rotation where there is none.

The illusion is created by the interaction between the light and dark regions of the image, which creates a sense of depth. The brain interprets the light and dark regions as if they were three-dimensional objects, and it uses the relative motion of the different parts of the image to perceive motion. The brain is tricked into perceiving rotation because the light and dark regions create the illusion of three-dimensional shapes that are moving relative to each other.

The Pinna illusion is one of the most powerful examples of kinetic illusions, and it is still not fully understood how it works. Some scientists have suggested that it may be related to the way the visual system processes information about the relative depth of different parts of an image, but more research is needed to fully understand the underlying mechanism of this illusion.

Version of the Pinna Illusion

The following are some alternate versions of the Pinna Illusion:

pinna-brelstaff illusion



pinna-brelstaff illusion



pinna-brelstaff illusion



Illusions like the Pinna Illusion

Kinetic illusions are visual illusions that involve motion. They work by exploiting the way our visual system processes information about motion. The brain uses a variety of cues, such as the relative motion of different parts of an image, to perceive motion.

Kinetic illusions take advantage of these cues by presenting images in a way that tricks the brain into perceiving motion where there is none, or perceiving motion in a different direction than what is actually happening

Some similar illusions are below:

The Fraser spiral illusion is an optical illusion that is characterized by the appearance of a spiral pattern made up of concentric circles.

The rotating snakes is a peripheral drift illusion that consists of a grid of shapes, with some of them appearing to be rotating or undulating. The illusion is created by the interaction of the shapes with the neural processing of the visual system.

Rotating Snakes Autokinetic effect
A Version of Rotating Snakes


The Moiré pattern illusion: This illusion is created by superimposing two similar patterns on top of each other, such as a grid of lines or circles. The resulting pattern appears to be moving or changing.

Moiré_pattern
From Wikimedia Commons

The Scintillating Grid Illusion, in which a grid of black and white squares appears to pulsate or “breathe” when viewed from the periphery of the image.

Simultaneous Contrast Illusion



The Hermann Grid Illusion in which the intersections of a white grid on a black background appear to be gray, even though they are actually the same color as the background.

Hermann-Grid Illusion


The Zöllner Illusion, in which parallel lines appear to be tilted or bent when intersected by diagonal lines.

Zöllner illusion


The barber pole illusion is an optical illusion that is characterized by the appearance of a spiral pattern on a vertically striped pole.


The Bezold Effect: This illusion is created by placing two or more colors next to each other, and the way they appear to change when they are close to one another.  

Bezold_Effect
from Wikimedia Commons


The Café Wall Illusion is a visual illusion that is created by a grid of alternating light and dark horizontal and vertical lines. The lines appear to be bent or tilted, even though they are actually straight.

Cafe Wall Illusion


Discovery of the Pinna Illusion

The Pinna Illusion is a visual illusion that was discovered by Bruno Pinna, a cognitive scientist and researcher in the field of visual perception.

Bruno Pinna is an Italian psychologist, known for his research in visual perception and his discovery of the Pinna illusion. He is currently a full professor of psychology at the University of Cagliari, Italy.

Pinna’s research focuses on the study of visual perception, in particular, the way in which the brain processes visual information and how it is influenced by the context of the visual environment. He is best known for his discovery of the Pinna illusion, which demonstrates the role of context and the importance of the visual cues provided by the surrounding environment in our perception of an object.

Pinna has published numerous papers on visual perception and the Pinna illusion in scientific journals, and he is considered a leading expert in the field of visual perception. He is also a member of various scientific societies and has been invited to give lectures and presentations on his research at conferences and universities around the world.

References and Resources

Check out our complete list of illusions.


Sign Up to Have Your Mind Blown

Pinna-Brelstaff Illusion

pinna-brelstaff illusion

The Pinna-Brelstaff Illusion is a visual illusion that demonstrates the role of context and the importance of the visual cues provided by the surrounding environment in our perception of an object. It is an adaptation of the original Pinna illusion.

The illusion is created by displaying a pattern of light and dark bars on a computer screen. The pattern appears to rotate, even though it is actually stationary.

pinna-brelstaff illusion
Pinna-Brelstaff Illusion


Table of Contents

How does the Pinna-Brelstaff Illusion work?

The Pinna-Brelstaff illusion is a kinetic illusion that involves the perception of motion in a still image. It was discovered by two researchers, Bruno Pinna and Tim Brelstaff in 2001. The illusion is created by displaying a pattern of light and dark bars on a computer screen. The pattern appears to rotate, even though it is actually stationary.

The illusion is created by the interaction between the light and dark bars, which creates a sense of depth, and the way that the brain processes information about motion. The brain interprets the light and dark bars as if they were three-dimensional objects, and it uses the relative motion of the different parts of the image to perceive motion. The brain is tricked into perceiving rotation because the light and dark bars create the illusion of three-dimensional shapes that are moving relative to each other.

This illusion can be explained by the theory of “perception of relative motion” the brain relies on the relative motion of the parts of an image to perceive motion. The light and dark bars creates the illusion of 3D shapes that are moving relative to each other, this creates the perception of rotation.

Version of the Pinna-Brelstaff Illusion

The following are some alternate versions of the Pinna-Brelstaff Illusion:



pinna-brelstaff illusion



pinna-brelstaff illusion



Illusions like the Pinna-Brelstaff Illusion

Kinetic illusions are visual illusions that involve motion. They work by exploiting the way our visual system processes information about motion. The brain uses a variety of cues, such as the relative motion of different parts of an image, to perceive motion.

Kinetic illusions take advantage of these cues by presenting images in a way that tricks the brain into perceiving motion where there is none, or perceiving motion in a different direction than what is actually happening

Some similar illusions are below:

The Fraser spiral illusion is an optical illusion that is characterized by the appearance of a spiral pattern made up of concentric circles.

The rotating snakes is a peripheral drift illusion that consists of a grid of shapes, with some of them appearing to be rotating or undulating. The illusion is created by the interaction of the shapes with the neural processing of the visual system.

Rotating Snakes Autokinetic effect
A Version of Rotating Snakes


The Moiré pattern illusion: This illusion is created by superimposing two similar patterns on top of each other, such as a grid of lines or circles. The resulting pattern appears to be moving or changing.

Moiré_pattern
From Wikimedia Commons

The Scintillating Grid Illusion, in which a grid of black and white squares appears to pulsate or “breathe” when viewed from the periphery of the image.

Simultaneous Contrast Illusion



The Hermann Grid Illusion in which the intersections of a white grid on a black background appear to be gray, even though they are actually the same color as the background.

Hermann-Grid Illusion


The Zöllner Illusion, in which parallel lines appear to be tilted or bent when intersected by diagonal lines.

Zöllner illusion


The barber pole illusion is an optical illusion that is characterized by the appearance of a spiral pattern on a vertically striped pole.


The Bezold Effect: This illusion is created by placing two or more colors next to each other, and the way they appear to change when they are close to one another.  

Bezold_Effect
from Wikimedia Commons


The Café Wall Illusion is a visual illusion that is created by a grid of alternating light and dark horizontal and vertical lines. The lines appear to be bent or tilted, even though they are actually straight.

Cafe Wall Illusion


Discovery of the Pinna-Brelstaff Illusion

It was discovered by two researchers, Bruno Pinna and Tim Brelstaff in 2001.

Bruno Pinna is an Italian psychologist, known for his research in visual perception and his discovery of the Pinna illusion. He is currently a full professor of psychology at the University of Cagliari, Italy.

Pinna’s research focuses on the study of visual perception, in particular, the way in which the brain processes visual information and how it is influenced by the context of the visual environment. He is best known for his discovery of the Pinna illusion, which demonstrates the role of context and the importance of the visual cues provided by the surrounding environment in our perception of an object.

Pinna has published numerous papers on visual perception and the Pinna illusion in scientific journals, and he is considered a leading expert in the field of visual perception. He is also a member of various scientific societies and has been invited to give lectures and presentations on his research at conferences and universities around the world.

References and Resources

Check out our complete list of illusions.


Sign Up to Have Your Mind Blown

Sander Illusion

Sander Illusion

In the Sander Illusion, the diagonal line on the left appears to be considerably longer than the diagonal line on the right, but the lines are the same length.

Sanders illusion
Sander Illusion


Table of Contents


How does the Sander Illusion work?

The Sander illusion works by exploiting the brain’s tendency to interpret the visual information it receives in the context of other visual information.

This illusion is thought to be caused by the visual system’s use of depth cues, such as the size and angle of the lines

The perception of depth and distance are affected by the context of the arrowheads, which fool the visual system into perceiving the lines as if they were in different distances and therefore different sizes.

Another explanation of the illusion is based on the concept of “Gestalt grouping” where the brain organizes different elements in a scene into groups based on certain rules such as proximity, similarity, and continuity. The triangles in the Sander illusion creates a sense of continuation and proximity between the lines and the triangleswhich leads to the perception of the lines as being different in length.


Versions of the Sander Illusion

The following are some alternate versions of the Sander Illusion:

Sanders hybrid
Sanders hybrid



Illusions like the Sander Illusion

There are many other visual perception illusions that work in similar ways to the Sander illusion. Some examples include:

The Müller-Lyer illusion is a visual perception illusion that consists of two lines of equal length, with arrows at each end pointing either inward or outward. The line with the inward-pointing arrows appears shorter than the line with the outward-pointing arrows, even though they are actually the same length.

müller-lyer illusion
The Müller-Lyer Illusion

The Poggendorff illusion: This illusion is similar to the Müller-Lyer illusion, but instead of arrowheads, it uses a diagonal line that intersects two parallel lines. The brain perceives the diagonal line as being tilted, causing the parallel lines to appear as if they are not parallel.

Poppendorff Illusion Lines


The Ebbinghaus illusion: This illusion involves circles of different sizes, but with the same-sized circles placed close to each other. The brain perceives the smaller circles as being larger when they are placed next to larger circles.

Ebbinghaus Illusion


The T illusion: This illusion involves a T-shaped figure with a horizontal bar and a vertical bar. The brain perceives the vertical bar as being longer than the horizontal bar, even though they are the same length.

T Illusion


The Kanizsa triangle: This illusion is created by placing Pac-Man-like shapes around an invisible triangle. The brain perceives the triangle as if it were a real, visible triangle.

Kanizsa Triangle Illusion


The Zöllner illusion: This illusion involves parallel lines with diagonal lines intersecting them. The brain perceives the lines as if they were not parallel and that the diagonal lines are converging.

Zöllner illusion


Moon illusion: This illusion involves the perception of the Moon appearing larger when it is near the horizon compared to when it is high in the sky.

Moon Illusion


Ponzo illusion: This illusion involves the perception of an object appearing larger or smaller depending on the size of the surrounding context.

Ponzo Illusion


The Café Wall Illusion is a visual illusion that is created by a grid of alternating light and dark horizontal and vertical lines. The lines appear to be bent or tilted, even though they are actually straight.

Cafe Wall Illusion


All these illusions are based on the brain’s perception of visual context, depth cues, and grouping rules. These illusions help to understand how the brain interprets visual information and how it can be deceived.

Discovery of the Sander Illusion

The illusion is named after German psychologist Friedrich Sander who described the illusion in 1926. However, the illusion was published earlier by Matthew Luckiesh.

Matthew Luckiesh was an American engineer and scientist who made significant contributions to the fields of lighting and optical engineering.

He was born in 1883 and died in 1967. He is known for his work on visual perception and the development of advanced lighting techniques, including the “daylight lamp” and the “sun lamp.”

He also wrote several books on the subject of light and vision, including “Artificial Light: Its Influence upon Civilization” and “The Science of Light.”

Luckiesh was a leading expert in his field and was considered one of the most important figures in the development of modern lighting technology.


References and Resources

Check out our complete list of illusions.


Sign Up to Have Your Mind Blown