Numerosity Adaptation Effect

The Numerosity adaptation effect refers to a phenomenon in which the perceived number of objects in a display is altered after exposure to a differently sized display.

The effect occurs when an individual is presented with a display of objects with a specific number density (number of objects per unit area) and then presented with a display of a different number density.

After exposure to the differently sized display, the individual’s perception of the number of objects in a subsequent display is altered and biased towards the number density of the adapted display.

The Numerosity adaptation effect is a demonstration of the plasticity of the visual system and how it can be influenced by experience. The effect has been widely studied in psychology and neuroscience and has been used to further our understanding of the underlying mechanisms of numerical perception and the role of experience in shaping perception.


Numerosity adaptation effect
Numerosity Adaptation Effect
From Wikimedia Commons


Table of Contents


How does the Numerosity Adaptation Effect work?

The Numerosity adaptation effect works by altering the sensitivity of the visual system to changes in the number density of objects in a display.

When an individual is exposed to a display of objects with a specific number density, the visual system adapts to that number density and becomes more sensitive to changes in that particular number density.

This leads to a bias in the perception of subsequent displays towards the number density of the adapted display.

The effect is thought to result from changes in the responsiveness of neurons in the visual cortex that are involved in representing the number of objects in a display.

The exact mechanism by which the Numerosity adaptation effect works is not well understood, but it is thought to involve changes in the gain or sensitivity of neural populations that are involved in processing numerical information.

The effect is thought to reflect a form of neural plasticity, where the visual system adapts to the statistics of the environment and changes the responsiveness of neurons accordingly.

This leads to a recalibration of the visual system’s sensitivity to changes in numerosity and a bias in the perception of subsequent displays.


Versions of the Numerosity Adaptation Effect

The following is an alternative Numerosity Adaptation Effect example:



Numerosity adaptation effect
Effect of Random vs. Organized Dots
All Circles on Left have the Same Number
All Circles on Right have the Same Number



Illusions like the Numerosity Adaptation Effect

The Numerosity adaptation effect is a type of perceptual illusion.

It is a demonstration of how our perception of the world can be influenced by experience and context. Perceptual illusions are situations where the sensory information that is being processed by the brain does not accurately reflect the physical reality of the stimulus.

The Numerosity adaptation effect is a form of illusion because it results in a misperception of the number of objects in a display, even though the physical number of objects has not changed. The effect is a result of changes in the responsiveness of neurons in the visual cortex that are involved in processing numerical information, and it reflects the plasticity and adaptability of the visual system.

Some related illusions include the following:

The missing square puzzle is a visual illusion that involves a square with a smaller square removed from one of its corners.

The puzzle is presented in such a way that the surrounding rectangles appear to be the same size, but when the lines of the squares are measured, it becomes clear that they are not.

The missing square puzzle is used to demonstrate the importance of context in visual perception and how our brain can be easily fooled into perceiving something that is not actually true.


Missing Square Puzzle
Missing Square Puzzle


Illusory contours are created by the brain’s interpretation of visual information, such as the arrangement and contrast of lines and shapes in the visual field.

Kanizsa Triangle Illusion
The Kanizsa Triangle

The Muller-Lyer illusion: an illusion in which two lines of the same length appear to be of different lengths due to the presence of arrowheads at the ends of the lines.


müller-lyer illusion



The Poggendorff illusion: an illusion in which two lines that are parallel in reality appear to be skewed or converging due to the presence of a third shape in the foreground.


Poggendorff illusion transparent gray bar



The Café Wall illusion: an illusion in which the lines that form a tiled wall appear to be skewed or distorted due to the presence of contrasting colored tiles.

Cafe Wall Illusion



The Zöllner illusion: an illusion in which parallel lines appear to be skewed or converging due to the presence of intersecting lines at an angle.

Zöllner illusion

The Ebbinghaus illusion: This illusion involves circles of different sizes, but with the same-sized circles placed close to each other. The brain perceives the smaller circles as being larger when they are placed next to larger circles.

Ebbinghaus Illusion


The T illusion: This illusion involves a T-shaped figure with a horizontal bar and a vertical bar. The brain perceives the vertical bar as being longer than the horizontal bar, even though they are the same length.


T Illusion


Moon illusion: This illusion involves the perception of the Moon appearing larger when it is near the horizon compared to when it is high in the sky.


Moon Illusion



Ponzo illusion: This illusion involves the perception of an object appearing larger or smaller depending on the size of the surrounding context.

Ponzo Illusion


All these illusions work by exploiting the way the brain processes visual information and the way it interprets the relationship between different elements in an image. Illusory contours are thought to be created by the activity of neurons that respond selectively to edges and contours in the visual field, which can be influenced by different visual cues.


Discovery of the Numerosity Adaptation Effect

The Numerosity adaptation effect was first documented in the psychological and neuroscience literature in the 1990s and has since been widely studied. It has been covered in some detail by Stanislas Dehaene.

The exact origin of the effect and who first discovered it is unclear, as multiple studies and researchers have contributed to our understanding of the effect.

The Numerosity adaptation effect is a result of the collective efforts of psychologists, neuroscientists, and other researchers who have worked to better understand the mechanisms of numerical perception and the role of experience in shaping perception.

The effect is a demonstration of the dynamic and adaptable nature of the visual system, and its discovery has helped to further our understanding of the underlying mechanisms of visual perception.

References and Resources

Check out our complete list of illusions.

Sign Up to Have Your Mind Blown