Vertical Horizontal Illusion

T Illusion Vertical–horizontal illusion

The Vertical–Horizontal illusion, also known as the T illusion, is a visual perception illusion that involves a T-shaped figure with a horizontal bar and a vertical bar. The brain perceives the vertical bar as being longer than the horizontal bar, even though they are the same length.

T Illusion Vertical–horizontal illusion
The Vertical–horizontal Illusion

Table of Contents


How does the Vertical–Horizontal Illusion work?


The T-illusion works by exploiting the brain’s tendency to interpret visual information in the context of other visual information. The brain uses various cues, such as angles and lines, to infer the three-dimensional structure of an object and the context in which it is presented. In the case of the T-illusion, the brain interprets the vertical bar as being farther away than the horizontal bar.

The brain uses the concept of linear perspective, which is the way that parallel lines appear to converge as they extend into the distance. The brain expects that the lines that are farther away should appear to be closer together than the lines that are closer. In the T-illusion, the vertical bar is interpreted as a continuation of the horizontal bar and the brain perceives it as farther away, which leads to the perception that the vertical bar is longer than the horizontal bar.

This illusion is also thought to be related to the concept of “Gestalt grouping” which is the way the brain organizes different elements in a scene into groups based on certain rules such as proximity, similarity, and continuity. The horizontal and vertical lines in the T-illusion create a sense of continuation and proximity between the lines which leads to the perception of the vertical line as a continuation of the horizontal line, making it appear longer.

In summary, the T-illusion works by the brain’s interpretation of visual information in the context of other visual information, the concept of linear perspective, and the Gestalt grouping rules. The brain uses these cues to perceive the lines as if they were in different distances and therefore different sizes.


Versions of the Vertical–Horizontal Illusion


The following are some alternate versions of the Vertical–Horizontal Illusion:

t illusion red and blue Vertical–horizontal illusion
T illusion blue Vertical–horizontal illusion


Illusions like the Vertical–Horizontal Illusion


The Poggendorff illusion: This illusion is similar to the Müller-Lyer illusion, but instead of arrowheads, it uses a diagonal line that intersects two parallel lines. The brain perceives the diagonal line as being tilted, causing the parallel lines to appear as if they are not parallel.

Poppendorff Illusion Lines



The Ebbinghaus illusion: This illusion involves circles of different sizes, but with the same-sized circles placed close to each other. The brain perceives the smaller circles as being larger when they are placed next to larger circles.


Ebbinghaus Illusion



The Müller-Lyer illusion is a visual perception illusion that consists of two lines of equal length, with arrows at each end pointing either inward or outward. The line with the inward-pointing arrows appears shorter than the line with the outward-pointing arrows, even though they are actually the same length. 

müller-lyer illusion



The Kanizsa triangle: This illusion is created by placing Pac-Man-like shapes around an invisible triangle. The brain perceives the triangle as if it were a real, visible triangle.

Kanizsa Triangle Illusion


The Zöllner illusion: This illusion involves parallel lines with diagonal lines intersecting them. The brain perceives the lines as if they were not parallel and that the diagonal lines are converging.

Zöllner illusion


Moon illusion: This illusion involves the perception of the Moon appearing larger when it is near the horizon compared to when it is high in the sky.

Moon Illusion



Ponzo illusion: This illusion involves the perception of an object appearing larger or smaller depending on the size of the surrounding context.

Ponzo Illusion



The Café Wall Illusion is a visual illusion that is created by a grid of alternating light and dark horizontal and vertical lines. The lines appear to be bent or tilted, even though they are actually straight.

Cafe Wall Illusion


All these illusions are based on the brain’s perception of visual context, depth cues, and grouping rules. These illusions help to understand how the brain interprets visual information and how it can be deceived.

Discovery of the Vertical–Horizontal Illusion

In 1858 Wilhelm Wundt introduced the horizontal-vertical illusion.

Wilhelm Wundt (1832-1920) was a German physician, physiologist, and philosopher. He is considered to be one of the founders of modern psychology and is often referred to as the “father of experimental psychology.” He is credited with establishing the first psychology laboratory in 1879 at the University of Leipzig in Germany, where he conducted research on the nature of mental processes and the relationship between the mind and the body.

Wundt’s work laid the foundation for the development of scientific psychology as a separate discipline. He believed that psychology should be studied using methods and techniques from the natural sciences, and he developed a number of experimental techniques to study mental processes such as sensation, perception, and attention. He also developed the concept of “introspection,” which is the process of self-observation and self-examination of one’s own mental states.

Wundt’s work had a major influence on the development of the field of psychology and his ideas continue to be studied and debated today. He is considered one of the most influential figures in the history of psychology and his legacy is still felt in the field today.

Wundt died in 1920 in Großbothen, Germany. He has written many books and articles on the topic of psychology, his most famous being “Principles of Physiological Psychology” which is considered as a cornerstone in the field of psychology.


Gender Differences with the Vertical–Horizontal Illusion

There is some research that suggests that there may be gender differences in the perception of the T-illusion. Some studies have found that men tend to perceive the vertical bar as longer than the horizontal bar more than women do. Other studies have found that women tend to perceive the horizontal bar as longer than the vertical bar more than men do.

There are a few possible explanations for these gender differences in the perception of the T-illusion. One possibility is that there may be differences in the way that men and women process visual information. For example, men may be more likely to rely on visual cues related to depth and distance, while women may be more likely to rely on cues related to shape and form.

Another possibility is that the gender differences in the perception of the T-illusion may be due to cultural or societal factors. Men and women may be exposed to different types of visual information and may have different experiences that affect their perception of the illusion.

It’s important to note that these gender differences in the perception of the T-illusion are not conclusive and more research is needed to understand the underlying cause and nature of these differences.

It’s also worth noting that despite any potential difference, the T-illusion is still a visual perception illusion that works by exploiting the brain’s tendency to interpret visual information in the context of other visual information and it’s an important tool for researchers to better understand how the brain process visual information.

Cultural Differences with the Vertical–Horizontal Illusion

There is some research that suggests that there may be cultural differences in the perception of the T-illusion. Some studies have found that people from different cultures perceive the T-illusion differently. For example, a study found that Westerners tend to perceive the vertical bar as longer than the horizontal bar, while people from some non-Western cultures tend to perceive the horizontal bar as longer than the vertical bar.

The explanations for these cultural differences in the perception of the T-illusion are not clear and there are multiple hypotheses, one of them is that cultural differences in visual perception may be related to differences in the way that people from different cultures process visual information. For example, people from Western cultures may be more likely to rely on visual cues related to depth and distance, while people from some non-Western cultures may be more likely to rely on cues related to shape and form.

Another explanation is related to the cultural context and the type of visual information people are exposed to, which may influence how they perceive visual information. For example, cultures that rely more on oral storytelling may be less affected by the illusion than cultures where visual representation is more prevalent.

It’s also possible that these cultural differences in the perception of the T-illusion may be due to other factors such as education, socioeconomic status, and exposure to different types of visual information.

It’s important to note that these cultural differences in the perception of the T-illusion are not conclusive, and more research is needed to understand the underlying causes and nature of these differences.

It’s also worth noting that despite any potential difference, the T-illusion is still a visual perception illusion that works by exploiting the brain’s tendency to interpret visual information in the context of other visual information and it’s an important tool for researchers to better understand how the brain processes visual information.


References and Resources

Check out our complete list of illusions.


Sign Up to Have Your Mind Blown

T Illusion

T Illusion Vertical–horizontal illusion

The T illusion, also known as the Vertical–horizontal illusion, is a visual perception illusion that involves a T-shaped figure with a horizontal bar and a vertical bar. The brain perceives the vertical bar as being longer than the horizontal bar, even though they are the same length.

T Illusion Vertical–horizontal illusion
The T Illusion

Table of Contents


How does the T Illusion work?


The T-illusion works by exploiting the brain’s tendency to interpret visual information in the context of other visual information. The brain uses various cues, such as angles and lines, to infer the three-dimensional structure of an object and the context in which it is presented. In the case of the T-illusion, the brain interprets the vertical bar as being farther away than the horizontal bar.

The brain uses the concept of linear perspective, which is the way that parallel lines appear to converge as they extend into the distance. The brain expects that the lines that are farther away should appear to be closer together than the lines that are closer. In the T-illusion, the vertical bar is interpreted as a continuation of the horizontal bar and the brain perceives it as farther away, which leads to the perception that the vertical bar is longer than the horizontal bar.

This illusion is also thought to be related to the concept of “Gestalt grouping” which is the way the brain organizes different elements in a scene into groups based on certain rules such as proximity, similarity, and continuity. The horizontal and vertical lines in the T-illusion create a sense of continuation and proximity between the lines which leads to the perception of the vertical line as a continuation of the horizontal line, making it appear longer.

In summary, the T-illusion works by the brain’s interpretation of visual information in the context of other visual information, the concept of linear perspective, and the Gestalt grouping rules. The brain uses these cues to perceive the lines as if they were in different distances and therefore different sizes.


Versions of the T Illusion


The following are some alternate versions of the T Illusion:

t illusion red and blue Vertical–horizontal illusion
T illusion blue Vertical–horizontal illusion


Illusions like the T Illusion


The Poggendorff illusion: This illusion is similar to the Müller-Lyer illusion, but instead of arrowheads, it uses a diagonal line that intersects two parallel lines. The brain perceives the diagonal line as being tilted, causing the parallel lines to appear as if they are not parallel.

Poppendorff Illusion Lines



The Ebbinghaus illusion: This illusion involves circles of different sizes, but with the same-sized circles placed close to each other. The brain perceives the smaller circles as being larger when they are placed next to larger circles.


Ebbinghaus Illusion



The Müller-Lyer illusion is a visual perception illusion that consists of two lines of equal length, with arrows at each end pointing either inward or outward. The line with the inward-pointing arrows appears shorter than the line with the outward-pointing arrows, even though they are actually the same length. 

müller-lyer illusion



The Kanizsa triangle: This illusion is created by placing Pac-Man-like shapes around an invisible triangle. The brain perceives the triangle as if it were a real, visible triangle.

Kanizsa Triangle Illusion


The Zöllner illusion: This illusion involves parallel lines with diagonal lines intersecting them. The brain perceives the lines as if they were not parallel and that the diagonal lines are converging.

Zöllner illusion


Moon illusion: This illusion involves the perception of the Moon appearing larger when it is near the horizon compared to when it is high in the sky.

Moon Illusion



Ponzo illusion: This illusion involves the perception of an object appearing larger or smaller depending on the size of the surrounding context.

Ponzo Illusion



The Café Wall Illusion is a visual illusion that is created by a grid of alternating light and dark horizontal and vertical lines. The lines appear to be bent or tilted, even though they are actually straight.

Cafe Wall Illusion


All these illusions are based on the brain’s perception of visual context, depth cues, and grouping rules. These illusions help to understand how the brain interprets visual information and how it can be deceived.

Discovery of the T Illusion

In 1858 Wilhelm Wundt introduced the horizontal-vertical illusion.

Wilhelm Wundt (1832-1920) was a German physician, physiologist, and philosopher. He is considered to be one of the founders of modern psychology and is often referred to as the “father of experimental psychology.” He is credited with establishing the first psychology laboratory in 1879 at the University of Leipzig in Germany, where he conducted research on the nature of mental processes and the relationship between the mind and the body.

Wundt’s work laid the foundation for the development of scientific psychology as a separate discipline. He believed that psychology should be studied using methods and techniques from the natural sciences, and he developed a number of experimental techniques to study mental processes such as sensation, perception, and attention. He also developed the concept of “introspection,” which is the process of self-observation and self-examination of one’s own mental states.

Wundt’s work had a major influence on the development of the field of psychology and his ideas continue to be studied and debated today. He is considered one of the most influential figures in the history of psychology and his legacy is still felt in the field today.

Wundt died in 1920 in Großbothen, Germany. He has written many books and articles on the topic of psychology, his most famous being “Principles of Physiological Psychology” which is considered as a cornerstone in the field of psychology.


Gender Differences with the T Illusion

There is some research that suggests that there may be gender differences in the perception of the T-illusion. Some studies have found that men tend to perceive the vertical bar as longer than the horizontal bar more than women do. Other studies have found that women tend to perceive the horizontal bar as longer than the vertical bar more than men do.

There are a few possible explanations for these gender differences in the perception of the T-illusion. One possibility is that there may be differences in the way that men and women process visual information. For example, men may be more likely to rely on visual cues related to depth and distance, while women may be more likely to rely on cues related to shape and form.

Another possibility is that the gender differences in the perception of the T-illusion may be due to cultural or societal factors. Men and women may be exposed to different types of visual information and may have different experiences that affect their perception of the illusion.

It’s important to note that these gender differences in the perception of the T-illusion are not conclusive and more research is needed to understand the underlying cause and nature of these differences.

It’s also worth noting that despite any potential difference, the T-illusion is still a visual perception illusion that works by exploiting the brain’s tendency to interpret visual information in the context of other visual information and it’s an important tool for researchers to better understand how the brain process visual information.

Cultural Differences with the T Illusion

There is some research that suggests that there may be cultural differences in the perception of the T-illusion. Some studies have found that people from different cultures perceive the T-illusion differently. For example, a study found that Westerners tend to perceive the vertical bar as longer than the horizontal bar, while people from some non-Western cultures tend to perceive the horizontal bar as longer than the vertical bar.

The explanations for these cultural differences in the perception of the T-illusion are not clear and there are multiple hypotheses, one of them is that cultural differences in visual perception may be related to differences in the way that people from different cultures process visual information. For example, people from Western cultures may be more likely to rely on visual cues related to depth and distance, while people from some non-Western cultures may be more likely to rely on cues related to shape and form.

Another explanation is related to the cultural context and the type of visual information people are exposed to, which may influence how they perceive visual information. For example, cultures that rely more on oral storytelling may be less affected by the illusion than cultures where visual representation is more prevalent.

It’s also possible that these cultural differences in the perception of the T-illusion may be due to other factors such as education, socioeconomic status, and exposure to different types of visual information.

It’s important to note that these cultural differences in the perception of the T-illusion are not conclusive, and more research is needed to understand the underlying causes and nature of these differences.

It’s also worth noting that despite any potential difference, the T-illusion is still a visual perception illusion that works by exploiting the brain’s tendency to interpret visual information in the context of other visual information and it’s an important tool for researchers to better understand how the brain processes visual information.


References and Resources

Check out our complete list of illusions.


Sign Up to Have Your Mind Blown

Müller-Lyer Illusion

müller-lyer illusion

The Müller-Lyer illusion is a visual perception illusion that consists of two lines of equal length, with arrows at each end pointing either inward or outward. The line with the inward-pointing arrows appears shorter than the line with the outward-pointing arrows, even though they are actually the same length. This illusion is often used in research on visual perception and the neural basis of perception.

müller-lyer illusion
The Müller-Lyer Illusion


Table of Contents


How does the Müller-Lyer Illusion work?

The Müller-Lyer illusion works by exploiting the brain’s tendency to interpret the visual information it receives in the context of other visual information. In the case of the Müller-Lyer illusion, the brain interprets the lines with inward-pointing arrows as being part of a concave shape, and the lines with outward-pointing arrows as being part of a convex shape. This interpretation leads the brain to perceive the lines with inward-pointing arrows as being shorter than they actually are, and the lines with outward-pointing arrows as being longer than they actually are.

This illusion is thought to be caused by the visual system’s use of depth cues, such as the size and angle of the arrowheads, to infer the three-dimensional structure of the lines. The visual system uses these cues to perceive the lines as being part of a 3D figure and not just a 2D representation. The perception of depth and distance are affected by the context of the arrowheads, which fool the visual system into perceiving the lines as if they were in different distances and therefore different sizes.

Another explanation of the illusion is based on the concept of “Gestalt grouping” where the brain organizes different elements in a scene into groups based on certain rules such as proximity, similarity, and continuity. The arrowheads in the Müller-Lyer illusion creates a sense of continuation and proximity between the lines and the arrowheads which leads to the perception of the lines as being different in length.


Versions of the Müller-Lyer Illusion

The following are some alternate versions of the Müller-Lyer Illusion:

Sarcone_Sinusoiral_Muller_Lyer_Illusion
Courtesy of Wikimedia Commons
Sarcones_Pulsating_Star_Dynamic_Muller-Lyer_illusion
Courtesy of Wikimedia Commons


Muller-Lyer-illusion-side-by-side-and-broken
muller-lyer-illusion-red-and-blue-lines




muller-lyer-illusion-circle-black-lines
muller-lyer-illusion-blue-lines-sharp-angle


muller-lyer-illusion-inside-rooms
Muller-Lyer-illusion-in-Box-Room


Illusions like the Müller-Lyer Illusion

There are many other visual perception illusions that work in similar ways to the Müller-Lyer illusion. Some examples include:

The Poggendorff illusion: This illusion is similar to the Müller-Lyer illusion, but instead of arrowheads, it uses a diagonal line that intersects two parallel lines. The brain perceives the diagonal line as being tilted, causing the parallel lines to appear as if they are not parallel.

Poppendorff Illusion Lines


The Ebbinghaus illusion: This illusion involves circles of different sizes, but with the same-sized circles placed close to each other. The brain perceives the smaller circles as being larger when they are placed next to larger circles.

Ebbinghaus Illusion


The T illusion: This illusion involves a T-shaped figure with a horizontal bar and a vertical bar. The brain perceives the vertical bar as being longer than the horizontal bar, even though they are the same length.

T Illusion


The Kanizsa triangle: This illusion is created by placing Pac-Man-like shapes around an invisible triangle. The brain perceives the triangle as if it were a real, visible triangle.

Kanizsa Triangle Illusion


The Zöllner illusion: This illusion involves parallel lines with diagonal lines intersecting them. The brain perceives the lines as if they were not parallel and that the diagonal lines are converging.

Zöllner illusion


Moon illusion: This illusion involves the perception of the Moon appearing larger when it is near the horizon compared to when it is high in the sky.

Moon Illusion


Ponzo illusion: This illusion involves the perception of an object appearing larger or smaller depending on the size of the surrounding context.

Ponzo Illusion


The Café Wall Illusion is a visual illusion that is created by a grid of alternating light and dark horizontal and vertical lines. The lines appear to be bent or tilted, even though they are actually straight.

Cafe Wall Illusion


All these illusions are based on the brain’s perception of visual context, depth cues, and grouping rules. These illusions help to understand how the brain interprets visual information and how it can be deceived.

Discovery of the Müller-Lyer Illusion

The Müller-Lyer illusion is a visual perception illusion that was first described by Franz Carl Müller-Lyer in 1889.

Franz Carl Müller-Lyer (1857-1916) was a German sociologist, psychologist, and philosopher. He is best known for his work on visual perception and the Müller-Lyer illusion, which is named after him. He studied at the University of Heidelberg and later at the University of Leipzig, where he received his Ph.D. in 1879.

In 1882, he published a book “Visual illusions: their causes, characteristics, and applications,” in which he described and illustrated the Müller-Lyer illusion, which is considered one of the most famous illusions in the field of psychology. He also wrote several books on the topic of social psychology, including “The People of Primitive Societies” and “The Social Order.”

Müller-Lyer was also a social reformer and advocated for better housing and living conditions for the working class. He was also interested in the relationship between society and the individual and his work aimed to bridge the gap between the social sciences, such as sociology and psychology.

Müller-Lyer died in 1916 in Munich, Germany. His work on the Müller-Lyer illusion and his contributions to the field of social psychology are still widely studied today.

References and Resources

Check out our complete list of illusions.


Sign Up to Have Your Mind Blown

Hering Illusion

Hering Illusion

The Hering illusion is a visual illusion that involves the perception of curved lines as being bowed inwards or outwards. In the illusion, the red lines appear to curve. In realty, the red lines are perfectly straight.

Hering Illusion
The Hering Illusion

Table of Contents


How does the Hering Illusion work?

The Hering illusion is a visual illusion that involves the perception of curved lines as being bowed inwards or outwards. The illusion is named after the German physiologist Ewald Hering, who first described it in 1861. The illusion is often used to study the perception of depth and the role of the visual system in shaping our perception of the world.

The illusion is created by placing a series of parallel lines in the form of a rectangle, with the lines on one side of the rectangle being closer together than the lines on the other side. This creates the illusion that the lines on one side of the rectangle are bowed inwards, while the lines on the other side appear to be bowed outwards.

The perception of the illusion is thought to be caused by the brain’s tendency to perceive the lines as being farther away or closer based on their spacing. The lines that are closer together are perceived as being farther away, while the lines that are farther apart are perceived as being closer. This leads to the perception of the lines as curved, when in reality they are straight.

The Hering illusion is used to demonstrate the role that context and surrounding plays in shaping our perception of reality. It also highlights how the brain uses depth cues such as relative size, distance, and position to perceive the 3D world around us.

Versions of the Hering Illusion

The following are some alternate versions of the Hering Illusion:

Hering-Illusion-alternate-version-square-background
Red Lines Appear to Bend
Hering-Illusion-alternate-version-square-background-removed
With background removed,
red lines are perfectly straight
Hering-Illusion-alternate-version-circle-background
Red Lines appear to bend
Hering-Illusion-alternate-version-circle-background-removed
With background removed,
red lines are perfectly straight

Illusions like the Hering Illusion

The following are some illusions similar to the Hering Illusion.

The Zöllner illusion is a visual illusion in which parallel lines appear to be angled due to the presence of intersecting lines.

The illusion is often used to study the brain’s perception of shape and spatial relationships. It is considered one of the most powerful and striking examples of a geometrical-optical illusion. The perception of the illusion can be explained by the brain’s tendency to group lines together based on their similarity in direction and spacing, which can lead to an overestimation of the angle between the parallel lines.

Zöllner illusion


The Müller-Lyer illusion: This illusion involves lines with arrowheads at the ends, which can make a line appear longer or shorter depending on the direction of the arrowheads.

müller-lyer illusion


The Poggendorff illusion is a visual illusion in which the brain perceives a diagonal line as being interrupted by an object, even though the line is actually continuous. The illusion is created by the misalignment of two parallel lines that are intersected by a third line at a certain angle.

Poggendorff illusion transparent gray bar


The Orbison illusion is an illusion of movement that is created when stationary, parallel lines are placed on a background of radiating lines. The parallel lines appear to be moving in the direction of the radiating lines, even though they are actually stationary. This illusion is thought to be caused by the interaction between the parallel lines and the radiating lines, which creates an illusion of movement in the brain.

Orbison Illusion


The Wundt illusion is an optical illusion produces an inversed effect compared to the Hering Illusion. The vertical lines are both straight, but they may look as if they are curved inwards.

Wundt Illusion


Discovery of the Hering Illusion

The illusion is named after the German physiologist Ewald Hering, who first described it in 1861.

Ewald Hering was a German physiologist and psychologist who lived in the 19th and early 20th centuries. He is best known for his research on perception and visual illusions, particularly the Hering illusion, which is named after him.

Hering’s work in perception focused on the process of color vision and binocular depth perception, and he proposed several theories to explain these phenomena. He also made important contributions to the study of visual illusions, and was one of the first researchers to systematically study the factors that influence the perception of depth and distance.

In addition to his research, Hering was also an influential teacher and mentor to many other researchers in the field of perception and vision. He held positions at several universities in Germany, including the University of Berlin and the University of Würzburg, where he trained many students who went on to make their own contributions to the field.

References and Resources

Check out our complete list of illusions.

.

Sign Up to Have Your Mind Blown

Zöllner Illusion

Zöllner illusion

The Zöllner illusion is a visual illusion in which parallel lines appear to be angled due to the presence of intersecting lines.

The illusion is often used to study the brain’s perception of shape and spatial relationships. It is considered one of the most powerful and striking examples of a geometrical-optical illusion. The perception of the illusion can be explained by the brain’s tendency to group lines together based on their similarity in direction and spacing, which can lead to an overestimation of the angle between the parallel lines.

Zöllner illusion
The Zöllner Illusion

Table of Contents

How does the Zöllner Illusion work?

The Zöllner illusion works by creating conflicting cues in the visual system. The parallel lines in the illusion are surrounded by intersecting lines that are angled, which causes the brain to perceive the parallel lines as angled as well.

The brain uses several cues, such as the angles of the intersecting lines, the spacing between the lines, and the relative size of the shapes to perceive the angle of the parallel lines.

When the brain receives conflicting cues, it can sometimes make errors in perception. In the case of the Zöllner illusion, the brain perceives the parallel lines as angled because the intersecting lines are angled, even though the parallel lines themselves are not.

This illusion helps to demonstrate that our perception of reality is not always accurate, and can be influenced by the context and surrounding of the visual information.

Versions of the Zöllner Illusion

The following are some alternate versions of the XX Illusion:

zollner illusion alternate version red black
zollner illusion alternate version black white horizontal

zollner illusion alternate version red black horizontal
zollner illusion alternate version black white diagonal

Illusions like the Zöllner Illusion

Illusions like the Zöllner illusion are known as geometric-optical illusions. These types of illusions are created by manipulating shapes and patterns in order to create conflicting cues in the visual system. This can cause the brain to perceive the shapes and patterns in a way that is different from reality. Some examples of other geometric-optical illusions include:

The Müller-Lyer illusion: This illusion involves lines with arrowheads at the ends, which can make a line appear longer or shorter depending on the direction of the arrowheads.

müller-lyer illusion

The Poggendorff illusion: This illusion involves two parallel lines that are interrupted by a third line or shape. The interruption can cause the parallel lines to appear angled, even though they are not.

Poggendorff illusion transparent gray bar

The Kanizsa triangle illusion: This illusion involves an illusory triangle that appears to be present in an image, even though it is not actually there. The triangle is created by the brain’s tendency to perceive shapes that are suggested by the placement of other shapes.

Kanizsa Triangle Illusion

The Hering Illusion: A visual illusion that involves the perception of curved lines as being bowed inwards or outwards.

Hering Illusion

The Café Wall Illusion is a visual illusion that is created by a grid of alternating light and dark horizontal and vertical lines. The lines appear to be bent or tilted, even though they are actually straight.

Cafe Wall Illusion

These illusions demonstrate the complex nature of visual perception and the role that context and surrounding can play in shaping our perception of reality.

Discovery of the Zöllner Illusion

The illusion was first described by German physicist and astronomer Johann Karl Friedrich Zöllner in 1860.

ohann Karl Friedrich Zöllner was a German physicist and astronomer, born in 1834 and died in 1882. He is best known for his work on visual perception, specifically for his discovery of the Zöllner illusion. He was a professor of physics at the University of Leipzig, where he performed many of his experiments on visual perception.

Zöllner was also interested in the relationship between science and spirituality, and attempted to use scientific methods to demonstrate the existence of spiritual phenomena such as ghosts and telepathy. He was a proponent of the idea that the material and spiritual worlds were connected and that scientific methods could be used to investigate spiritual phenomena.

Zöllner’s work on visual perception was influential in the field of psychology, and his discovery of the Zöllner illusion is still widely studied and used in research today. He is considered as a pioneer of the field of neuroscience and cognitive psychology.

He also made other important contributions in the field of physics, such as the design of a polarimeter, the invention of a spark-spectroscope, the study of the polarisation of light and the discovery of the phenomenon of “Zöllner lines”.

References and Resources

Check out our complete list of illusions.

Sign Up to Have Your Mind Blown

Delboeuf Illusion

Delboeuf illusion Large

The Delboeuf illusion is a perceptual illusion in which the perceived size of a circle is affected by the size of the surrounding circles.

In the illusion, a central circle surrounded by larger circles appears smaller than the same central circle surrounded by smaller circles, even though the central circle is identical in both cases. The illusion demonstrates that the perceived size of an object can be influenced by the context in which it is presented.

Delboeuf illusion Large
The Delboeuf Illusion

Table of Contents

How does the Delboeuf Illusion work?

The Delboeuf illusion works by altering the perceived relative size of the central circle compared to the surrounding circles. The brain uses the size of the surrounding circles as a reference point to judge the size of the central circle. When the surrounding circles are larger, the central circle appears smaller in comparison, and when the surrounding circles are smaller, the central circle appears larger.

This illusion is thought to be caused by a process in the brain called “size constancy scaling.” This process allows the brain to adjust for the size of an object relative to its surroundings. In the Delboeuf illusion, the brain incorrectly adjusts the perceived size of the central circle based on the size of the surrounding circles.

It’s also thought that the Delboeuf illusion is caused by the way the brain processes the spatial relationships between objects. The brain uses distance and angle as cues to determine the size of an object, and these cues can be influenced by the presence of surrounding objects. In the case of the Delboeuf illusion, the brain may interpret the larger surrounding circles as being closer to the central circle, making the central circle appear smaller.

It’s important to note that the Delboeuf illusion is not limited to circles, it works also with other shapes and sizes, it’s a general principle that applies to all types of stimuli.

Versions of the Delboeuf Illusion

The following are some versions of the Delbouf Illusion

Delboeuf circle with lines
Delboeuf Illusion Circles
Delboeuf Illusion Food Pasta
Delboeuf Illusion Circle Square

Illusions like the Delboeuf Illusion

The Delboeuf Illusion is a size constancy illusions in which the perceived size of an object is affected by its surroundings. Some examples of size constancy illusions include:

The Ebbinghaus illusion: This illusion is similar to the Delboeuf illusion but in this case the central circle appears larger when surrounded by smaller circles and smaller when surrounded by larger circles.

Ebbinghaus Illusion

The Occlusion illusion is a visual illusion where an object that is partially obscured by another object appears to be farther away than it actually is. This illusion is caused by the brain’s interpretation of the relative depth of the objects based on their relative size and position. This phenomenon is based on the visual cues that the brain uses to perceive depth, such as relative size, position, and overlap.

The occlusion illusion

The Müller-Lyer illusion is a classic example of size-contrast illusion, in which two lines of equal length appear to be different due to the presence of arrowheads or fins at the end of the lines.

müller-lyer illusion

Moon illusion: This illusion involves the perception of the Moon appearing larger when it is near the horizon compared to when it is high in the sky.

Moon Illusion

Ponzo illusion: This illusion involves the perception of an object appearing larger or smaller depending on the size of the surrounding context.

Ponzo Illusion

All these illusions demonstrate that the perceived size of an object can be influenced by the context in which it is presented, and that the brain uses the size of the surrounding objects as a reference point to judge the size of the central object.

Discovery of the Delboeuf Illusion

The illusion is named after the Belgian psychologist Joseph Delboeuf, who first described it in 1865.

Joseph Remi Leopold Delboeuf (1831-1896) was a Belgian psychologist and philosopher who is best known for his work on the Delboeuf illusion, a perceptual illusion in which the perceived size of a circle is affected by the size of the surrounding circles. Delboeuf was one of the early pioneers of experimental psychology, and his work on the Delboeuf illusion was an important contribution to our understanding of how the brain processes visual information.

Delboeuf was born in Brussels, Belgium in 1831. He studied mathematics, physics and chemistry at the University of Brussels and later received a degree in medicine. He then studied philosophy and psychology at the Universities of Leipzig and Berlin. After completing his education, he returned to Brussels and became a professor of psychology at the University of Brussels. He also founded the Belgian Society for Psychology.

Delboeuf’s work on the Delboeuf illusion was published in 1865, in which he presented the illusion and demonstrated that the perceived size of an object can be influenced by the context in which it is presented. His work in this area was important in showing that visual perception is not just a passive process, but that the brain actively interprets the information it receives. He also made other contributions to the field of psychology, he studied the perception of time, and he wrote on the philosophy of psychology.

Delboeuf died in 1896 in Brussels, Belgium. His legacy in psychology is still being studied and cited today.

Impact of the Delboeuf Illusion in the Food Industry

The Delboeuf illusion impacts the food industry in a few ways.

First, the illusion can be used to make food portions appear larger or smaller than they actually are. For example, using smaller plates or bowls can make a small portion of food appear larger, while using larger plates or bowls can make a large portion of food appear smaller. This can be used to create the illusion of value for customers, or to make healthier portion sizes appear more satisfying.

Second, the Delboeuf illusion can be used in food packaging design. For example, using larger or smaller images of food on packaging can make the contents of the package appear larger or smaller than they actually are. This can be used to make smaller products appear more appealing or to make larger products appear more economical.

Third, the Delboeuf illusion can be used in restaurant settings. For example, using smaller plates or glasses can make small servings of food or drink appear larger, while using larger plates or glasses can make larger servings appear smaller. This can be used to influence customer perceptions of value and satisfaction.

It’s important to note that in the food industry, manipulating the Delboeuf illusion can be considered as a form of deceptive marketing, and in some countries, it is regulated by law. It’s important to be transparent and honest with the customers and to follow the laws.

References and Resources

Check out our complete list of illusions.

Sign Up to Have Your Mind Blown

Poggendorff Illusion

Poggendorff illusion Red Blue Black Lines

The Poggendorff illusion is a visual illusion in which the brain perceives a diagonal line as being interrupted by an object, even though the line is actually continuous.

The illusion is created by the misalignment of two parallel lines that are intersected by a third line at a certain angle.

In the version below, a straight black and red line is covered by grey rectangle. However, the blue line, not red line, appears to be an extension of the black line. The image on the right reveals the truth.

Poggendorff illusion Red Blue Black Lines
The Poggendorff Illusion

Table of Contents

How does the Poggendorff Illusion work?

The Poggendorff illusion works by the brain interpreting the misalignment of two parallel lines that are intersected by a third line at a certain angle as a gap or interruption in the diagonal line, even though the line is actually continuous. It is thought that the brain interprets the misalignment as an occlusion, or an object blocking the diagonal line, which causes the illusion.

The exact neural mechanisms that underlie the Poggendorff illusion are not well understood, but it is thought that the brain’s tendency to group similar elements together, known as gestalt grouping, plays a role. Additionally, the brain’s tendency to perceive lines and edges as continuing behind occluding objects may also contribute to the illusion.

Overall, the Poggendorff illusion demonstrate that visual perception is an active process that is influenced by both the physical properties of the stimulus and the brain’s assumptions about the scene.

The illusion was first described by the German physicist Johann Poggendorff in 1860. It is often used in research on perception and cognitive psychology to study the way the brain processes visual information.

Versions of the Poggendorff Illusion

The following are some alternate versions of the Poggendorff Illusion

Poggendorff illusion transparent gray bar
poggendorff illusion example blocks of wood
Poggendorff illusion blank space
Poggendorff illusion multiple lines

Illusions like the Poggendorff Illusion

The Poggendorff illusion is a type of size-distance illusions, also known as size contrast illusions, which are visual illusions in which the size or distance of an object is perceived differently than it actually is. These illusions are caused by the way the brain processes visual information and the surrounding context. Some examples of size-distance illusions include:

The Ebbinghaus illusion (also known as Titchener circles) is a perceptual illusion in which the perceived size of a central circle is affected by the size of the surrounding circles. The central circle appears smaller when surrounded by larger circles, and larger when surrounded by smaller circles.

Ebbinghaus Illusion

The Delboeuf Illusion: This illusion involves the perception of circles of different sizes appearing to be the same size when they are surrounded by other circles of different sizes.

Delboeuf Illusion
delboeuf illusion with food.

The Zöllner illusion: A another size-contrast illusion in which a group of diagonal lines appear to be distorted or bent due to the presence of intersecting lines. This illusion involves the perception of diagonal lines that appear to be bent or tilted, even though they are actually straight.

Zöllner illusion

The Müller-Lyer illusion is a classic example of size-contrast illusion, in which two lines of equal length appear to be different due to the presence of arrowheads or fins at the end of the lines.

müller-lyer illusion

Moon illusion: This illusion involves the perception of the Moon appearing larger when it is near the horizon compared to when it is high in the sky.

Moon Illusion

Ponzo illusion: This illusion involves the perception of an object appearing larger or smaller depending on the size of the surrounding context.

Ponzo Illusion

The Café Wall Illusion  is a visual illusion that is created by a grid of alternating light and dark horizontal and vertical lines. The lines appear to be bent or tilted, even though they are actually straight.

Cafe Wall Illusion

Münsterberg illusion: A special version of the Café Wall Illusion. This illusion involving the perception of the size of an object appearing to change when the surrounding context changes.

Münsterberg Illusion

These illusions show that the brain’s perception of size and distance can be influenced by the surrounding context, and that the visual information the brain receives is not always an accurate representation of reality.

Discovery of the Poggendorff Illusion

The illusion was first described by the German physicist Johann Poggendorff in 1860. It is often used in research on perception and cognitive psychology to study the way the brain processes visual information.

Johann Christian Poggendorff was a German physicist, mathematician and inventor. He is best known for his work in the field of electricity and magnetism, and for his invention of the Poggendorff Illusion. He was born in 1796 in Hamburg, Germany and died in 1877 in Berlin, Germany.

Poggendorff studied mathematics and physics at the University of Berlin and later worked as a professor of physics at the University of Breslau. He made significant contributions to the field of electricity and magnetism, including the invention of the Poggendorff Illusion in 1860. He also developed the Poggendorff’s circle, a mathematical construct used to represent the intersection of two circles in a plane.

Poggendorff was also a member of several scientific societies and was awarded several honors for his work, including the Copley Medal of the Royal Society of London in 1878. He is also remembered for his contributions to the field of science through the Poggendorff’s Annalen der Physik und Chemie, which he founded and edited for many years.

References and Resources

Check out our complete list of illusions.

Sign Up to Have Your Mind Blown

Ebbinghaus Illusion

Ebbinghaus Illusion Cropped

The Ebbinghaus illusion (also known as Titchener circles) is a perceptual illusion in which the perceived size of a central circle is affected by the size of the surrounding circles. The central circle appears smaller when surrounded by larger circles, and larger when surrounded by smaller circles.

Ebbinghaus Illusion
The Ebbinghaus Illusion

Table of Contents


How does the Ebbinghaus Illusion work?

The Ebbinghaus illusion works by exploiting the way our brain processes visual information. Our brain is constantly trying to make sense of the visual information it receives by making comparisons and judgments about the size, shape, and position of objects in our environment. In the case of the Ebbinghaus illusion, the brain is making a comparison between the size of the central circle and the size of the surrounding circles.

When the surrounding circles are larger than the central circle, our brain perceives them as being farther away and thus the central circle appears smaller in comparison. When the surrounding circles are smaller than the central circle, our brain perceives them as being closer and the central circle appears larger in comparison.

Additionally, the brain also takes into account the proximity of the surrounding circles to the central circle. So, if the surrounding circles are placed in close proximity to the central circle, it will appear smaller than when they are placed farther away.

This illusion is thought to be caused by a neural mechanism in the brain called contextual modulation, which modifies the visual perception of an object based on the context in which it is presented. The Ebbinghaus illusion is a classic example of how context can affect perception and highlights the complex nature of visual processing in the brain.

Versions of the Ebbinghaus Illusion

The following are some alternate versions of the Ebbinghaus Illusion

Ebbinghaus Illusion Alternate Version with Lines
Ebbinghaus-Illusion-Alternate-Version
Ebbinghaus Illusion Alternate Version Circle
Ebbinghaus Illusion Alternate Version Titchener Circle

Illusions like the Ebbinghaus Illusion

The Ebbinghaus illusion is a type of size-contrast illusion. A size-contrast illusion is a type of visual illusion in which the perceived size of an object is affected by the size of the surrounding objects. The Ebbinghaus illusion specifically is related to relative size, where the perception of the size of the central circle changes depending on the size of the surrounding circles. Size-contrast illusions occur due to the way that the visual system processes relative size information in a scene, and the Ebbinghaus illusion is a classic example of this type of illusion.

There are many other examples of size-contrast illusions in addition to the Ebbinghaus illusion. Here are a few examples:

The Delboeuf illusion is similar to the Ebbinghaus illusion, but instead of circles, it uses two concentric circles or rings. The central ring appears larger or smaller depending on the size of the surrounding ring.

Delboeuf Illusion
delboeuf illusion with food.

The Poggendorff illusion is another size-contrast illusion in which a straight line appears to be bent or angled, due to the presence of intersecting lines or shapes in the background.

Poppendorff Illusion Lines

The Zöllner illusion is another size-contrast illusion in which a group of diagonal lines appear to be distorted or bent due to the presence of intersecting lines.

Zöllner illusion


The Müller-Lyer illusion is a classic example of size-contrast illusion, in which two lines of equal length appear to be different due to the presence of arrowheads or fins at the end of the lines.

müller-lyer illusion

All of these illusions rely on the way the visual system processes relative size information, and they all highlight the complexity and malleability of visual perception.


Discovery of the Ebbinghaus Illusion

This illusion is named after German psychologist Hermann Ebbinghaus, who first described it in the late 19th century. The illusion is thought to be caused by the way the brain processes relative size information in visual scenes.

Hermann Ebbinghaus (1850-1909) was a German psychologist who is best known for his pioneering research on memory and forgetting. He was one of the first researchers to systematically study memory as a psychological process and his work laid the foundation for the field of experimental psychology. Ebbinghaus conducted a series of experiments in which he memorized lists of nonsense syllables and then tested his own memory at various intervals to measure the rate of forgetting.

He also developed the first standardized memory test, called the “memory drum”, which consisted of a rotating drum with lists of words or syllables that could be presented to participants at different intervals. He used these tests to study the effects of different factors, such as repetition and interference, on memory retention.

Ebbinghaus also made significant contributions to the understanding of memory processes, such as the spacing effect, which states that spaced practice is more effective than massed practice in enhancing memory retention.

Ebbinghaus’s work was groundbreaking in the field of psychology and is still widely cited today. The Ebbinghaus illusion, the size-contrast illusion that bears his name, was discovered by him in 1885 but is not as well-known as his memory research.

References and Resources

Check out our complete list of illusions.

Sign Up to Have Your Mind Blown