Leaning Tower Illusion

Leaning Tower Illusion

The Leaning Tower Illusion is an optical illusion that involves the perception of the tilt angle of a tower or building.

In this illusion, although the images are duplicates, one has the impression that the tower on the right leans more, as if photographed from a different angle.

Leaning Tower Illusion
Leaning Tower Illusion

Table of Contents

How does the Leaning Tower Illusion work?

The Leaning Tower Illusion is thought to be caused by the way the brain processes the visual information it receives.

The brain uses information from various cues, such as the relative size and orientation of objects in the scene, to determine the perspective and depth of the objects.

The Leaning Tower Illusion is a well-known example of how the brain’s interpretation of visual information can be influenced by context and how our perception of the world can be influenced by the way the brain processes information.

It continues to be widely studied and used as a demonstration of the power of optical illusions and the role of context in shaping our perception of the world.

Versions of the Leaning Tower Illusion

The following is a version of the Leaning Tower Illusion with Big Ben:


Leaning Tower Illusion - Big Ben





Illusions like the Leaning Tower Illusion

Leaning Tower Illusion is an optical illusion that manipulates the viewer’s perception of depth, scale, and distance.

The brain’s perception of size is influenced by depth cues such as perspective and accommodation, leading to an incorrect assessment of the object’s size.

This type of illusion is created through the use of visual cues, such as size, position, and relative proportions, that trick the brain into perceiving the scene in a certain way.

Some similar illusions are as follows:

Forced perspective is a technique used in photography, architecture, and other visual arts to manipulate the perception of the size and distance of objects.

It creates the illusion of a larger or smaller object, or of one that is closer or farther away, by carefully controlling the angles, proportions, and placement of objects in the scene.

The Ponzo illusion is a type of visual illusion in which a pair of parallel lines, or a horizontal bar, appear to be of different lengths based on the presence of a converging or diverging set of lines, often resembling the converging parallel lines of a road or railway track, placed above and below them.

Ponzo illusion
The Ponzo Illusion

The Ebbinghaus illusion: This illusion is similar to the Delboeuf illusion but in this case the central circle appears larger when surrounded by smaller circles and smaller when surrounded by larger circles.

Ebbinghaus Illusion

The Delboeuf illusion is a perceptual illusion in which the perceived size of a circle is affected by the size of the surrounding circles.


The Occlusion illusion is a visual illusion where an object that is partially obscured by another object appears to be farther away than it actually is. This illusion is caused by the brain’s interpretation of the relative depth of the objects based on their relative size and position. This phenomenon is based on the visual cues that the brain uses to perceive depth, such as relative size, position, and overlap.

The occlusion illusion

The Müller-Lyer illusion is a classic example of size-contrast illusion, in which two lines of equal length appear to be different due to the presence of arrowheads or fins at the end of the lines.

müller-lyer illusion

The moon illusions involves the perception of the Moon appearing larger when it is near the horizon compared to when it is high in the sky.

The Kanizsa triangle is a visual illusion that was first described by the Italian psychologist Gaetano Kanizsa in 1955. The illusion consists of a white equilateral triangle that appears to be surrounded by three Pac-Man-like shapes, which are black and have white crescents facing inwards.

All these illusions demonstrate that the perceived size of an object can be influenced by the context in which it is presented, and that the brain uses the size of the surrounding objects as a reference point to judge the size of the central object.

Discovery of the Leaning Tower of Pisa Illusion

No single individual is credited with the discovery of this illusion of the Leaning Tower of Pisa

References and Resources

Check out our complete list of illusions.

Sign Up to Have Your Mind Blown

Hollow Faced Illusion

Hollow Faced

The hollow face illusion is a type of optical illusion where a concave mask appears as if it is a convex object.

When a person views a hollow mask from the front, the features on the surface of the mask appear to be bulging out, even though they are actually recessed.

This illusion is created by the brain’s interpretation of the visual information it receives from the eyes. The brain uses previous experiences and knowledge to make assumptions about the shape and position of objects in a scene, and in the case of the hollow face illusion, these assumptions lead to a misinterpretation of the actual shape of the mask.

Hollow Faced
Hollow Faced Illusion
From Wikimedia Commons

Table of Contents


How does the Hollow Faced Illusion work?

The hollow face illusion works by tricking the brain’s interpretation of visual information from the eyes. When viewing a concave mask from the front, the brain uses past experiences and knowledge to assume that the features on the surface of the mask should appear to be recessed. However, due to the specific way in which the mask is designed, the features on the surface appear to be bulging out.

This illusion is created through the use of visual cues, such as shadows, shading, and contours, that the brain uses to determine the depth and position of objects in a scene. The brain uses these cues to construct a 3D representation of the scene, but in the case of the hollow face illusion, the cues are misleading, causing the brain to misinterpret the actual shape of the mask.

Studies have shown that the hollow face illusion is stronger in people who have experience handling and perceiving 3D objects, as they have developed a stronger representation of 3D shapes in their brain. Additionally, the strength of the illusion can also be affected by factors such as the lighting conditions and the viewer’s distance from the mask.

Versions of the Hollow Faced Illusion

The following is another version of the Hollow Faced Illusion. in the image, the face appears pushed out, but is actually pushed in.

Hollow Faced



Illusions like the Hollow Faced Illusion

The following are some illusions similar to the Hollow Faced Illusion.

The Rotating Snakes is a peripheral drift illusion that consists of a grid of shapes, with some of them appearing to be rotating or undulating. The illusion is created by the interaction of the shapes with the neural processing of the visual system.

Rotating Snakes Autokinetic effect


The Moiré pattern illusion: This illusion is created by superimposing two similar patterns on top of each other, such as a grid of lines or circles. The resulting pattern appears to be moving or changing.

Moiré_pattern
From Wikimedia Commons


The Scintillating Grid Illusion, in which a grid of black and white squares appears to pulsate or “breathe” when viewed from the periphery of the image.

Simultaneous Contrast Illusion


The Zöllner illusion is a visual illusion in which parallel lines appear to be angled due to the presence of intersecting lines.

The illusion is often used to study the brain’s perception of shape and spatial relationships. It is considered one of the most powerful and striking examples of a geometrical-optical illusion. The perception of the illusion can be explained by the brain’s tendency to group lines together based on their similarity in direction and spacing, which can lead to an overestimation of the angle between the parallel lines.

Zöllner illusion


The Müller-Lyer illusion: This illusion involves lines with arrowheads at the ends, which can make a line appear longer or shorter depending on the direction of the arrowheads.

müller-lyer illusion



The Poggendorff illusion is a visual illusion in which the brain perceives a diagonal line as being interrupted by an object, even though the line is actually continuous. The illusion is created by the misalignment of two parallel lines that are intersected by a third line at a certain angle.

Poggendorff illusion transparent gray bar



The Cafe Wall Illusion is a geometric optical illusion that is created by the alignment of parallel lines in a checkerboard pattern. The parallel lines appear to be tilted or slanted, even though they are actually straight.

Cafe Wall Illusion



The Wundt illusion is an optical illusion produces an inversed effect compared to the Hering Illusion. The vertical lines are both straight, but they may look as if they are curved inwards.

Wundt Illusion



Discovery of the Hollow Faced Illusion

The exact origin of the hollow face illusion is not known, but it is a phenomenon that has been studied and discussed by psychologists and neuroscientists for many years.

The illusion was first described in the scientific literature in the 1930s, and since then, it has been the subject of numerous studies and experiments aimed at understanding the neural and cognitive mechanisms underlying depth perception and 3D shape representation.

While it is not possible to attribute the discovery of the hollow face illusion to a single person, the concept has been developed and explored by many researchers and scientists over the years.

The hollow face illusion was first described by the psychologist James J. Gibson in the 1930s. He was one of the first scientists to study the phenomenon and provide a detailed description of the illusion and its underlying mechanisms. In his work, Gibson emphasized the importance of the visual information that the eyes receive from the environment in shaping our perception of the world, and he suggested that the hollow face illusion was an example of how the brain uses this information to construct a 3D representation of the world.

Gibson’s work on the hollow face illusion was influential in the development of the ecological theory of perception, which argues that perception is not solely determined by the properties of objects themselves, but is also shaped by the context in which they are encountered and the information available in the environment. His contributions to the study of the hollow face illusion and the broader field of perception and cognitive psychology continue to be widely recognized and referenced in the field today.

The study of this illusion continues to be an active area of research, with new findings and insights being added to our understanding of the brain’s ability to perceive and interpret visual information.

References and Resources

Check out our complete list of illusions and the related Pyramid of Giza illusion

.

Forced Perspective

Forced Perspective

Forced perspective is a technique used in photography, architecture, and other visual arts to manipulate the perception of the size and distance of objects.

It creates the illusion of a larger or smaller object, or of one that is closer or farther away, by carefully controlling the angles, proportions, and placement of objects in the scene.

Forced Perspective
Forced Perspective

Table of Contents

How does Forced Perspective work?

Forced perspective works by using optical illusions to manipulate the viewer’s perception of depth, scale, and distance.

This is achieved by playing with the relative sizes and positions of objects in a scene, and by using techniques like converging lines, foreshortening, and atmospheric perspective.

For example, a building designed in forced perspective may appear to be taller than it actually is, by gradually reducing the size of its features as they extend upward.

This trick of the eye is accomplished by making the upper part of the building smaller in proportion to the lower part, while maintaining the same perspective lines.

Our brain relies on past experiences and knowledge to make sense of the visual information, and in this case, the context of the converging lines creates an expectation of depth and distance, which in turn affects perception.

Versions of the Forced Perspective

The following are some examples of Forced Perspective


Forced Perspective



Forced Perspective




Forced Perspective



Illusions like the Forced Perspective

Forced perspective is an optical illusion that manipulates the viewer’s perception of depth, scale, and distance.

It creates the illusion of a larger or smaller object, or of one that is closer or farther away, by carefully controlling the angles, proportions, and placement of objects in the scene.

This type of illusion is created through the use of visual cues, such as size, position, and relative proportions, that trick the brain into perceiving the scene in a certain way. The effect of forced perspective can be quite convincing, making it seem as if objects are actually different sizes or distances than they are in reality.

Some similar illusions are as follows:

The Ponzo illusion is a type of visual illusion in which a pair of parallel lines, or a horizontal bar, appear to be of different lengths based on the presence of a converging or diverging set of lines, often resembling the converging parallel lines of a road or railway track, placed above and below them.

Ponzo illusion
The Ponzo Illusion

The Ebbinghaus illusion: This illusion is similar to the Delboeuf illusion but in this case the central circle appears larger when surrounded by smaller circles and smaller when surrounded by larger circles.

Ebbinghaus Illusion

The Delboeuf illusion is a perceptual illusion in which the perceived size of a circle is affected by the size of the surrounding circles.


The Occlusion illusion is a visual illusion where an object that is partially obscured by another object appears to be farther away than it actually is. This illusion is caused by the brain’s interpretation of the relative depth of the objects based on their relative size and position. This phenomenon is based on the visual cues that the brain uses to perceive depth, such as relative size, position, and overlap.

The occlusion illusion

The Müller-Lyer illusion is a classic example of size-contrast illusion, in which two lines of equal length appear to be different due to the presence of arrowheads or fins at the end of the lines.

müller-lyer illusion

The moon illusions involves the perception of the Moon appearing larger when it is near the horizon compared to when it is high in the sky.

The Kanizsa triangle is a visual illusion that was first described by the Italian psychologist Gaetano Kanizsa in 1955. The illusion consists of a white equilateral triangle that appears to be surrounded by three Pac-Man-like shapes, which are black and have white crescents facing inwards.

All these illusions demonstrate that the perceived size of an object can be influenced by the context in which it is presented, and that the brain uses the size of the surrounding objects as a reference point to judge the size of the central object.

Discovery of the Forced Perspective

Forced perspective is a technique that has been used for thousands of years in various forms of art, architecture, and visual storytelling.

As such, it is not credited to a single person or time period. The earliest known examples of forced perspective date back to ancient Egyptian and Greek architecture, where columns and other architectural elements were designed to create the illusion of greater height or size.

Over time, the use of forced perspective has evolved and expanded, with artists and architects in many different cultures and time periods incorporating it into their work in different ways.

It is a technique that continues to be used today in many forms of art and media.

References and Resources

Check out our complete list of illusions.

Sign Up to Have Your Mind Blown

Crater Illusion

Crater Illusion

The crater illusion is a visual illusion that make it seem that craters appear to be raised, when if fact, they are indented.

Crater Illusion
The Crater Illusion

Table of Contents


How does the Crater Illusion work?

The illusion is created by the brain’s interpretation of depth and perspective. The circles appear to be raised or indented because the brain interprets the circles that are farther away as smaller and those that are closer as larger. This creates the illusion of depth and the appearance of a series of shallow or deep depressions.

The illusion is a variation of the well-known “Ebbinghaus illusion” which is based on the same principle of relative size perception, but it’s composed by circles instead of circles and circles and it’s called the “crater illusion” because the circles resemble the shape of craters.

It is important to note that the illusion is purely visual, the circles themselves are actually the same size, and there is no actual depth or depression.

Versions of the Crater Illusion

The following is another example of the Crater Illusion:


Crater Illusion

Illusions like the Crater Illusion

The crater illusion is a type of visual illusion known as a size-distance illusion. Size-distance illusions involve the perception of objects that appear to be different sizes based on their distance from the viewer. The crater illusion specifically is a variation of the well-known “Ebbinghaus illusion” which is a size-distance illusion that occurs when two circles of the same size are surrounded by circles of different sizes.

Size-distance illusions are caused by the way our brains perceive and interpret visual information. Our brains use various cues, such as relative size, texture, and lighting to judge the distance of objects in our environment. In the case of the crater illusion, the brain interprets the circles that are farther away as smaller and those that are closer as larger, which creates the illusion of depth and the appearance of a series of shallow or deep depressions.

It’s important to note that the illusion is purely visual, the circles themselves are actually the same size, and there is no actual depth or depression.

The Ebbinghaus illusion (also known as Titchener circles) is a perceptual illusion in which the perceived size of a central circle is affected by the size of the surrounding circles. The central circle appears smaller when surrounded by larger circles, and larger when surrounded by smaller circles.

Ebbinghaus Illusion


The Delboeuf illusion is similar to the Ebbinghaus illusion, but instead of circles, it uses two concentric circles or rings. The central ring appears larger or smaller depending on the size of the surrounding ring.

Delboeuf Illusion
delboeuf illusion with food.

The Poggendorff illusion is another size-contrast illusion in which a straight line appears to be bent or angled, due to the presence of intersecting lines or shapes in the background.

Poppendorff Illusion Lines

The Zöllner illusion is another size-contrast illusion in which a group of diagonal lines appear to be distorted or bent due to the presence of intersecting lines.

Zöllner illusion


The Müller-Lyer illusion is a classic example of size-contrast illusion, in which two lines of equal length appear to be different due to the presence of arrowheads or fins at the end of the lines.

müller-lyer illusion

All of these illusions rely on the way the visual system processes relative size information, and they all highlight the complexity and malleability of visual perception.


Discovery of the Crater Illusion

The origins of the crater illusion are not well-documented, but it is considered a variation of the well-known “Ebbinghaus illusion” which was first described by the German psychologist Hermann Ebbinghaus in his 1885 book “Über das Gedächtnis” (On Memory).

He observed that when two circles of the same size are surrounded by circles of different sizes, the central circles appear to be different sizes. This illusion is an example of the size-distance illusion, which occurs when the perception of the size of an object is affected by its distance from the viewer.

It’s important to note that the crater illusion, as a specific variation of the Ebbinghaus illusion, is a well-established phenomenon in the field of psychology and it’s widely studied by researchers in the field of visual perception.

The Ebbinghaus illusion is named after German psychologist Hermann Ebbinghaus, who first described it in the late 19th century. The illusion is thought to be caused by the way the brain processes relative size information in visual scenes.

Hermann Ebbinghaus (1850-1909) was a German psychologist who is best known for his pioneering research on memory and forgetting. He was one of the first researchers to systematically study memory as a psychological process and his work laid the foundation for the field of experimental psychology. Ebbinghaus conducted a series of experiments in which he memorized lists of nonsense syllables and then tested his own memory at various intervals to measure the rate of forgetting.

He also developed the first standardized memory test, called the “memory drum”, which consisted of a rotating drum with lists of words or syllables that could be presented to participants at different intervals. He used these tests to study the effects of different factors, such as repetition and interference, on memory retention.

Ebbinghaus also made significant contributions to the understanding of memory processes, such as the spacing effect, which states that spaced practice is more effective than massed practice in enhancing memory retention.

Ebbinghaus’s work was groundbreaking in the field of psychology and is still widely cited today. The Ebbinghaus illusion, the size-contrast illusion that bears his name, was discovered by him in 1885 but is not as well-known as his memory research.

References and Resources

Check out our complete list of illusions.

Ames Window

Ames Window

The Ames Window (or trapezoid) is a flat object that seems to be a rectangular window but is actually a trapezoid.

The illusion is created by manipulating the perspective through the use of angles.

The image below shows the Ames Window, but scroll down to the alternate versions and the video to see this amazing illusion in action.

Ames Window
Ames Window


Table of Contents


How does the Ames Window Illusion work?

The Ames Window is a type of illusion known as a perspective illusion, which is a visual illusion that occurs when the perceived size, shape, or position of an object is different from its actual size, shape, or position, due to the way that the eyes and brain process visual information.

Perspective illusions are created by manipulating the visual cues that the brain uses to perceive depth, such as the relative size and position of objects, the angle of the lines and edges, and the amount of light and shadow.

The Ames Window is a specific type of perspective illusion that uses the manipulation of perspective to create the illusion.


Versions of the Ames Window Illusion

The following are some alternate versions of the Ames Window Illusion:



Ames Window



Illusions like the Ames Window Illusion

There are many other visual perception illusions that work in similar ways to the Ames Window illusion. Some examples include:

The Ames Room illusion is a visual illusion in which a room appears distorted, causing people or objects within it to appear differently sized.

Ames Room Black and White
Ames Room

The Müller-Lyer illusion is a visual perception illusion that consists of two lines of equal length, with arrows at each end pointing either inward or outward. The line with the inward-pointing arrows appears shorter than the line with the outward-pointing arrows, even though they are actually the same length.

müller-lyer illusion
The Müller-Lyer Illusion

The Poggendorff illusion: This illusion is similar to the Müller-Lyer illusion, but instead of arrowheads, it uses a diagonal line that intersects two parallel lines. The brain perceives the diagonal line as being tilted, causing the parallel lines to appear as if they are not parallel.

Poppendorff Illusion Lines


The Ebbinghaus illusion: This illusion involves circles of different sizes, but with the same-sized circles placed close to each other. The brain perceives the smaller circles as being larger when they are placed next to larger circles.

Ebbinghaus Illusion


The T illusion: This illusion involves a T-shaped figure with a horizontal bar and a vertical bar. The brain perceives the vertical bar as being longer than the horizontal bar, even though they are the same length.

T Illusion


The Kanizsa triangle: This illusion is created by placing Pac-Man-like shapes around an invisible triangle. The brain perceives the triangle as if it were a real, visible triangle.

Kanizsa Triangle Illusion


The Zöllner illusion: This illusion involves parallel lines with diagonal lines intersecting them. The brain perceives the lines as if they were not parallel and that the diagonal lines are converging.

Zöllner illusion


Moon illusion: This illusion involves the perception of the Moon appearing larger when it is near the horizon compared to when it is high in the sky.

Moon Illusion


Ponzo illusion: This illusion involves the perception of an object appearing larger or smaller depending on the size of the surrounding context.

Ponzo Illusion


The Café Wall Illusion is a visual illusion that is created by a grid of alternating light and dark horizontal and vertical lines. The lines appear to be bent or tilted, even though they are actually straight.

Cafe Wall Illusion


All these illusions are based on the brain’s perception of visual context, depth cues, and grouping rules. These illusions help to understand how the brain interprets visual information and how it can be deceived.

Discovery of the Ames Window Illusion

The illusion was first described by American psychologist Adelbert Ames Jr. in 1947, and is often used in research on visual perception and optical illusions.

Ames was a professor of psychology at the University of Michigan and later at Harvard University.

He was known for his research on visual perception, optical illusions, and the role of the visual system in the perception of motion.

Ames was also known for his work on the psychology of perception in relation to art and design.

He published several papers and articles on the Ames Room illusion and its implications for understanding the visual system.

References and Resources

Check out our complete list of illusions.


Sign Up to Have Your Mind Blown

Ames Room

Ames Room Black and White

The Ames Room illusion is a visual illusion in which a room appears distorted, causing people or objects within it to appear differently sized.

The illusion is created by manipulating the perspective of the room through the use of angled walls and a skewed ceiling, which causes the viewer’s perception of depth and size to be altered.

Ames Room Black and White
Ames Room


Table of Contents


How does the Ames Room Illusion work?

The Ames Room illusion works by manipulating the perspective of the room in such a way that the viewer’s perception of depth and size is altered.

This is achieved by building the room with angled walls and a skewed ceiling, which causes the room to appear distorted.

The illusion is created by making one corner of the room appear closer to the viewer than the other, while keeping the size of the room constant.

This causes the objects or people within the room to appear differently sized, as the closer corner appears larger and the farther corner appears smaller.

The illusion is enhanced by placing a person or object in each corner and having them move, it will appear as if one is bigger than the other, even though they are the same size.

The illusion is so convincing that it can be difficult to believe that the room is not actually distorted, and it is often used in research on visual perception and optical illusions.


Versions of the Ames Room Illusion

The following are some alternate versions of the Ames Room Illusion:

Ames Room Image


Ames Room Diagram


Ames Room Diagram



Illusions like the Ames Room Illusion

he Ames Room is a type of illusion known as a perspective illusion, which is a visual illusion that occurs when the perceived size, shape, or position of an object is different from its actual size, shape, or position, due to the way that the eyes and brain process visual information.

Perspective illusions are created by manipulating the visual cues that the brain uses to perceive depth, such as the relative size and position of objects, the angle of the lines and edges, and the amount of light and shadow.

The Ames Room is a specific type of perspective illusion that uses the manipulation of the room’s perspective to create the illusion of differently sized people or objects within it.

The illusion is so convincing that it can be difficult to believe that the room is not actually distorted.

There are many other visual perception illusions that work in similar ways to the Ames Room illusion. Some examples include:

The Müller-Lyer illusion is a visual perception illusion that consists of two lines of equal length, with arrows at each end pointing either inward or outward. The line with the inward-pointing arrows appears shorter than the line with the outward-pointing arrows, even though they are actually the same length.

müller-lyer illusion
The Müller-Lyer Illusion

The Poggendorff illusion: This illusion is similar to the Müller-Lyer illusion, but instead of arrowheads, it uses a diagonal line that intersects two parallel lines. The brain perceives the diagonal line as being tilted, causing the parallel lines to appear as if they are not parallel.

Poppendorff Illusion Lines


The Ebbinghaus illusion: This illusion involves circles of different sizes, but with the same-sized circles placed close to each other. The brain perceives the smaller circles as being larger when they are placed next to larger circles.

Ebbinghaus Illusion


The T illusion: This illusion involves a T-shaped figure with a horizontal bar and a vertical bar. The brain perceives the vertical bar as being longer than the horizontal bar, even though they are the same length.

T Illusion


The Kanizsa triangle: This illusion is created by placing Pac-Man-like shapes around an invisible triangle. The brain perceives the triangle as if it were a real, visible triangle.

Kanizsa Triangle Illusion


The Zöllner illusion: This illusion involves parallel lines with diagonal lines intersecting them. The brain perceives the lines as if they were not parallel and that the diagonal lines are converging.

Zöllner illusion


Moon illusion: This illusion involves the perception of the Moon appearing larger when it is near the horizon compared to when it is high in the sky.

Moon Illusion


Ponzo illusion: This illusion involves the perception of an object appearing larger or smaller depending on the size of the surrounding context.

Ponzo Illusion


The Café Wall Illusion is a visual illusion that is created by a grid of alternating light and dark horizontal and vertical lines. The lines appear to be bent or tilted, even though they are actually straight.

Cafe Wall Illusion


All these illusions are based on the brain’s perception of visual context, depth cues, and grouping rules. These illusions help to understand how the brain interprets visual information and how it can be deceived.

Discovery of the Ames Room Illusion

The illusion was first described by American psychologist Adelbert Ames Jr. in the 1950s, and is often used in research on visual perception and optical illusions.

Ames was a professor of psychology at the University of Michigan and later at Harvard University.

He was known for his research on visual perception, optical illusions, and the role of the visual system in the perception of motion.

Ames was also known for his work on the psychology of perception in relation to art and design.

He published several papers and articles on the Ames Room illusion and its implications for understanding the visual system.

References and Resources

Check out our complete list of illusions.


Sign Up to Have Your Mind Blown

Sander Illusion

Sander Illusion

In the Sander Illusion, the diagonal line on the left appears to be considerably longer than the diagonal line on the right, but the lines are the same length.

Sanders illusion
Sander Illusion


Table of Contents


How does the Sander Illusion work?

The Sander illusion works by exploiting the brain’s tendency to interpret the visual information it receives in the context of other visual information.

This illusion is thought to be caused by the visual system’s use of depth cues, such as the size and angle of the lines

The perception of depth and distance are affected by the context of the arrowheads, which fool the visual system into perceiving the lines as if they were in different distances and therefore different sizes.

Another explanation of the illusion is based on the concept of “Gestalt grouping” where the brain organizes different elements in a scene into groups based on certain rules such as proximity, similarity, and continuity. The triangles in the Sander illusion creates a sense of continuation and proximity between the lines and the triangleswhich leads to the perception of the lines as being different in length.


Versions of the Sander Illusion

The following are some alternate versions of the Sander Illusion:

Sanders hybrid
Sanders hybrid



Illusions like the Sander Illusion

There are many other visual perception illusions that work in similar ways to the Sander illusion. Some examples include:

The Müller-Lyer illusion is a visual perception illusion that consists of two lines of equal length, with arrows at each end pointing either inward or outward. The line with the inward-pointing arrows appears shorter than the line with the outward-pointing arrows, even though they are actually the same length.

müller-lyer illusion
The Müller-Lyer Illusion

The Poggendorff illusion: This illusion is similar to the Müller-Lyer illusion, but instead of arrowheads, it uses a diagonal line that intersects two parallel lines. The brain perceives the diagonal line as being tilted, causing the parallel lines to appear as if they are not parallel.

Poppendorff Illusion Lines


The Ebbinghaus illusion: This illusion involves circles of different sizes, but with the same-sized circles placed close to each other. The brain perceives the smaller circles as being larger when they are placed next to larger circles.

Ebbinghaus Illusion


The T illusion: This illusion involves a T-shaped figure with a horizontal bar and a vertical bar. The brain perceives the vertical bar as being longer than the horizontal bar, even though they are the same length.

T Illusion


The Kanizsa triangle: This illusion is created by placing Pac-Man-like shapes around an invisible triangle. The brain perceives the triangle as if it were a real, visible triangle.

Kanizsa Triangle Illusion


The Zöllner illusion: This illusion involves parallel lines with diagonal lines intersecting them. The brain perceives the lines as if they were not parallel and that the diagonal lines are converging.

Zöllner illusion


Moon illusion: This illusion involves the perception of the Moon appearing larger when it is near the horizon compared to when it is high in the sky.

Moon Illusion


Ponzo illusion: This illusion involves the perception of an object appearing larger or smaller depending on the size of the surrounding context.

Ponzo Illusion


The Café Wall Illusion is a visual illusion that is created by a grid of alternating light and dark horizontal and vertical lines. The lines appear to be bent or tilted, even though they are actually straight.

Cafe Wall Illusion


All these illusions are based on the brain’s perception of visual context, depth cues, and grouping rules. These illusions help to understand how the brain interprets visual information and how it can be deceived.

Discovery of the Sander Illusion

The illusion is named after German psychologist Friedrich Sander who described the illusion in 1926. However, the illusion was published earlier by Matthew Luckiesh.

Matthew Luckiesh was an American engineer and scientist who made significant contributions to the fields of lighting and optical engineering.

He was born in 1883 and died in 1967. He is known for his work on visual perception and the development of advanced lighting techniques, including the “daylight lamp” and the “sun lamp.”

He also wrote several books on the subject of light and vision, including “Artificial Light: Its Influence upon Civilization” and “The Science of Light.”

Luckiesh was a leading expert in his field and was considered one of the most important figures in the development of modern lighting technology.


References and Resources

Check out our complete list of illusions.


Sign Up to Have Your Mind Blown

Jastrow Illusion

Jastrow

The Jastrow illusion is created by two identical shapes, usually circles, placed side by side, and one appears to be much larger.

The illusion is that the one circle appears to be much larger than the other, but in realty, the shapes are the same size.

Jastrow
The Jastrow Illusion

Table of Contents


How does the Jastrow Illusion work?

The Jastrow illusion works by the way the brain processes the relationship between the two circles.

One theory of why this happens is that the brain tries to find a relationship between the two circles, and when it sees that one is larger than the other, it automatically assumes that the larger circle is farther away and the smaller circle is closer. Because the brain perceives objects that are farther away as being smaller and objects that are closer as being larger, it overcompensates and perceives the larger circle as much larger than it actually is, and the smaller circle as much smaller than it actually is.

Another theory is that the brain is sensitive to the relative size of objects and relies on relative size to infer distance, which is known as size-distance illusion. When the two circles are placed side by side, the relative size of the circles can be used by the brain to infer depth and distance, and this leads to the illusion.

Additionally, the Jastrow illusion is also related to the concept of surround suppression, which is the phenomenon that the visual system is more sensitive to the stimuli in the center of the visual field, than at the periphery. The brain is more sensitive to the stimuli in the center and suppresses the stimuli in the periphery, leading to an illusion of size difference.

It’s worth noting that the Jastrow illusion is not a unique property of human vision, many animals have this ability as well.

Versions of the Jastrow Illusion

The following shows Jastrow Illusion in action:

Jastrow GIF



Illusions like the Jastrow Illusion

The Jastrow illusion is a type of size illusion, also known as a relative size illusion. It is created by placing two identical shapes, usually circles, that are slightly different in size side by side and connecting them with a line.

This illusion is caused by the way the brain processes the relationship between the two shapes, and it is a result of the brain’s tendency to use relative size as a cue for distance and depth perception.

The following are some illusions similar to the Jastrow Illusion:

The Cafe Wall Illusion is a geometric optical illusion that is created by the alignment of parallel lines in a checkerboard pattern. The parallel lines appear to be tilted or slanted, even though they are actually straight.


Cafe Wall Illusion
The Café Wall Illusion

The occlusion illusion is a perceptual phenomenon in which the presence or absence of an object can affect the perception of another object.



The moon illusion involves the perception of the Moon appearing larger when it is near the horizon compared to when it is high in the sky. The illusion is an optical illusion and is caused by the way the human brain perceives size and distance.



The Zöllner illusion is a visual illusion in which parallel lines appear to be angled due to the presence of intersecting lines.

The illusion is often used to study the brain’s perception of shape and spatial relationships. It is considered one of the most powerful and striking examples of a geometrical-optical illusion. The perception of the illusion can be explained by the brain’s tendency to group lines together based on their similarity in direction and spacing, which can lead to an overestimation of the angle between the parallel lines.

Zöllner illusion


The Müller-Lyer illusion: This illusion involves lines with arrowheads at the ends, which can make a line appear longer or shorter depending on the direction of the arrowheads.

müller-lyer illusion



The Poggendorff illusion is a visual illusion in which the brain perceives a diagonal line as being interrupted by an object, even though the line is actually continuous. The illusion is created by the misalignment of two parallel lines that are intersected by a third line at a certain angle.


Poggendorff illusion transparent gray bar


The Ebbinghaus illusion (also known as Titchener circles) is a perceptual illusion in which the perceived size of a central circle is affected by the size of the surrounding circles. The central circle appears smaller when surrounded by larger circles, and larger when surrounded by smaller circles.





The Wundt illusion is an optical illusion produces an inversed effect compared to the Hering Illusion. The vertical lines are both straight, but they may look as if they are curved inwards.

Wundt Illusion



Discovery of the Jastrow Illusion

The Jastrow illusion is a visual illusion that was first described by the psychologist Joseph Jastrow in 1889.

Joseph Jastrow was an American psychologist and author.

He was born in 1863 in Poland and immigrated to the United States with his family as a child.

He received his Ph.D. from Johns Hopkins University and later taught at the University of Wisconsin-Madison and at Brown University.

Jastrow made significant contributions to the field of psychology, particularly in the areas of perception and cognitive psychology.

He is best known for his work on visual illusions, including the Jastrow illusion which is named after him. Jastrow died in 1944.

References and Resources

Check out our complete list of illusions.

.

Sign Up to Have Your Mind Blown

Apparent Motion

Phenakistoscope

Apparent motion, sometimes called the phi phenomenon, is the visual illusion of motion created by the rapid presentation of a sequence of still images. It is based on the idea that our eyes and brain perceive a series of still images as a continuous stream of motion. Phi phenomenon is technically a specific case of apparent motion observed when two nearby stimuli are presented in alternation with a relatively high frequency.

The phi phenomenon is the basis for the stroboscopic effect, it is the illusion of motion created by the rapid succession of still images that makes the stroboscopic effect work, where the object appears to be frozen in a single position, even though it is in motion.

The phi phenomenon has been studied extensively in fields such as psychology, neuroscience, and cognitive science, and it continues to be an important area of research today.


Phenakistoscope
Apparent motion


Table of Contents


How does Apparent Motion work?

Apparent motion, also known as the phi phenomenon, works by taking advantage of the way our eyes and brain process visual information. The human visual system is designed to detect and respond to movement, and it does this by constantly analyzing the images it receives from the eyes.

When a sequence of still images are presented in quick succession, the visual system perceives the images as a single object moving from one position to another. This is because the visual system is constantly analyzing the images, and it uses the information from one image to make predictions about the next image. When these predictions are confirmed, the brain interprets this as a single object moving.

The speed of the apparent motion is determined by the rate at which the images are presented. The faster the rate, the smoother the apparent motion will appear. The brain also uses other cues such as brightness, size, and color to determine the apparent motion.

The phi phenomenon is based on the idea that our eyes and brain perceive a series of still images as a continuous stream of motion, this is the basis for the stroboscopic effect, where the object appears to be frozen in a single position, even though it is in motion, and it is also the basis for motion picture and animation, where a series of still images are projected in rapid succession to create the illusion of motion.

The Related and Often Confused Illusory Motion

Note that sometimes apparent motion is used to refer to illusory motion which is when static image appears to be moving due to the interaction of color contrasts, shapes, and position.

Illusory motion, also known as motion illusion, is the perception of movement in a static image or pattern.

This can occur when the brain is presented with a series of static images that change over time, or when certain visual patterns are arranged in a specific way.

The brain interprets these as if they are moving, even though the image or pattern itself is not actually moving.

There are several theories that try to explain the underlying mechanisms of illusory motion.

One theory is that it is a result of neural adaptation, where the visual system becomes adapted to certain stimuli and continues to respond as if they were present even after they are removed.

Another theory is that it is a result of the brain’s tendency to perceive patterns and to fill in missing information, known as Gestalt principles.

Versions of Apparent Motion

The following are some other examples of Apparent Motion


Apparent motion, also known as the phi phenomenon
Apparent motion
“phi phenomenon”




Phi_Phenomenon-Yellow-Circles-1.gif



Apparent motion, also known as the phi phenomenon
Apparent motion
“phi phenomenon”





Illusions like Apparent Motion

The following are some illusions related to Apparent Motion

In Peripheral Drift illusions, the image or pattern appears to move or drift, even though it is actually stationary. This movement is caused by the brain’s attempt to interpret the image or pattern, which is often complex or ambiguous. The movement can be in any direction, such as horizontally, vertically, or diagonally.

Peripheral drift illusion jelly bean
From Wikimedia Commons

The rotating snakes is a peripheral drift illusion that consists of a grid of shapes, with some of them appearing to be rotating or undulating. The illusion is created by the interaction of the shapes with the neural processing of the visual system.

Rotating Snakes Autokinetic effect
A Version of Rotating Snakes


With Illusory motion, static image appears to be moving due to the interaction of color contrasts, shapes, and position

Troxler’s fading, also known as Troxler’s effect, is a phenomenon in which a stationary visual stimulus eventually disappears from perception, even though it is still present in the visual field.

Lilac-Chaser_Troxlers Fading


The stroboscopic effect is a visual phenomenon that occurs when a rapidly flashing light is used to illuminate a moving object.

The object appears to be frozen in a single position, even though it is in motion. This is because the flashing light is only on for a brief period of time, and the object is only visible when the light is on.

Stroboscopic effect

The Scintillating Grid Illusion, in which a grid of black and white squares appears to pulsate or “breathe” when viewed from the periphery of the image.

Simultaneous Contrast Illusion

The Motion Aftereffect (also known as the Waterfall Illusion) is a visual illusion experienced after viewing a moving visual stimulus, and then fixating on a stationary stimulus. The stationary stimulus appears to move in the opposite direction to the original stimulus.

For example, fixate on the moving image below for several seconds, then shift you gaze to another surface like a plain wall. You should still be able to see the motion on the static surface.

Motion-Aftereffect



The Café Wall Illusion is a visual illusion that is created by a grid of alternating light and dark horizontal and vertical lines. The lines appear to be bent or tilted, even though they are actually straight.

Cafe Wall Illusion



These illusions are usually caused by the way our eyes process visual information and the way the brain interprets it. They can also be caused by the interaction of different visual elements, such as lines and angles, in the image. They are often used in research on visual perception and the neural basis of perception.


Discovery of the Apparent Motion

The phi phenomenon was first described by the German psychologist Max Wertheimer in 1912.

He observed that if two lights are turned on and off in quick succession, the brain perceives them as a single light moving back and forth. This is the basic principle behind motion pictures and animation, where a series of still images are projected in rapid succession to create the illusion of motion.

The phi phenomenon, also known as apparent motion, was first described by the German psychologist Max Wertheimer in his 1912 paper “Experimental Studies of the Perception of Movement.” In this paper, Wertheimer described a series of experiments in which he used a sequence of still images to create the illusion of motion, and he noted that the brain perceives these images as a single moving object.

Wertheimer’s work on the phi phenomenon was groundbreaking and had a significant impact on the field of psychology, particularly in the area of perception. His findings helped to establish the Gestalt school of psychology, which focuses on the way the brain organizes sensory information.

Wertheimer’s work on the phi phenomenon also laid the foundation for the study of motion perception and animation and helped to pave the way for the later development of motion pictures and television.

It’s worth noting that the phi phenomenon was also independently studied by other researchers such as Kurt Koffka and Wolfgang Köhler, who were also part of the Gestalt school of psychology and they made contributions to the field as well.


References and Resources

Check out our complete list of illusions.

Impossible Decision

An impossible Penrose Decision

An impossible decision on this Penrose Triangle…

The Penrose triangle, also known as the Penrose tribar, is an optical illusion that depicts a three-dimensional object that is physically impossible to construct.

An impossible Penrose Decision

The Penrose triangle, also known as the Penrose tribar, is an optical illusion that depicts a three-dimensional object that is physically impossible to construct.

The illusion is a two-dimensional drawing that is designed to create the impression of a triangular structure, but the angles of the lines are inconsistent with the laws of Euclidean geometry, making it impossible to construct in the real world.

Penrose Triangle
The Penrose Triangle Illusion

Table of Contents

How does the Penrose Triangle Illusion work?

The Penrose triangle works by exploiting the way the brain interprets the 2-dimensional image of a triangular structure, but with impossible angles and lines that are inconsistent with the laws of Euclidean geometry.

These impossible angles and lines create a visual illusion of depth and three-dimensionality that is impossible to construct in the real world.

The illusion works by playing with the viewer’s expectations about the relationships between the lines and angles of the object and the viewer’s sense of depth.

The brain uses cues such as perspective, shading, and past experiences to create a 3-dimensional perception of the image.

However, in the case of the Penrose triangle, the brain is unable to reconcile the conflicting information, creating the illusion of an impossible object.

The Penrose triangle illusion also highlights the concept of “perceptual set” which refers to the brain’s ability to focus on specific aspects of the image and ignore others depending on the context.

The brain tends to focus on the lines and angles that suggest depth and three-dimensionality, while ignoring the impossible angles and lines that would make the object impossible to construct in the real world.

Additionally, the concept of “top-down processing” comes into play as the brain uses prior knowledge to interpret the visual information coming from the eyes, and tries to make sense of the image with the information it has.

The brain is unable to reconcile the conflicting information in the Penrose triangle, creating the illusion of an impossible object.

Versions of the Penrose Triangle Illusion

The following are some alternate versions of the Penrose Triangle Illusion:

Penrose Triangle clored
From Wikimedia Commons



Penrose Triangle
From Wikimedia Commons



Penrose Triangle rainbow GIF
From Wikimedia Commons


Illusions like the Penrose Triangle Illusion

Paradox illusions are a type of optical illusion that involve images that appear to be self-contradictory or impossible. They typically involve the manipulation of visual cues such as size, shape, movement, and depth perception to create an image that appears to be impossible or defies our understanding of the physical world.

In general, these illusions work by exploiting the way the visual system processes information. The brain relies on certain cues, such as perspective, shading, and texture, to infer the 3D structure of an object. When these cues are manipulated in a certain way, the brain can be fooled into perceiving an impossible or self-contradictory image.

Some related illusions include the following:

The Rubin vase, also known as the Rubin face or the figure-ground vase, is a famous optical illusion in which the image of a vase can also be perceived as two faces in profile looking at each other.

Rubin Vase Classic Black and White


The duck-rabbit illusion is an optical illusion that is an image that can be perceived as either a duck or a rabbit depending on how the viewer looks at it.

Duck Rabbit Illusion

The Necker cube is an optical illusion that features a simple wireframe drawing of a cube. The cube appears to switch back and forth between two different orientations.

Necker Cube

The My Wife and My Mother-in-Law illusion is an optical illusion with an image of a young woman’s face that can also be perceived as an older woman’s face.

Wife and Mother In Law

The Schröder Staircase is an optical illusion that features a drawing of a staircase. The staircase appears to be either ascending or descending, depending on how the brain interprets the angles of the lines.

Schroeders_stairs
From Wikimedia Commons

The impossible cube is an optical illusion that depicts a three-dimensional object that is physically impossible to construct.

Impossible Cube Illusion


The impossible trident is a three-pronged impossible shape resembling a trident. It is usually depicted as a three-pronged fork with each prong appearing to be a continuation of the next, creating an impossible shape.

Impossible Trident
From Wikimedia Commons

The spinning dancer illusion is a visual illusion that depicts a silhouette of a dancer spinning clockwise or counterclockwise. The direction of the dancer’s spin can appear to change depending on the viewer’s perception

Spinning Dancer Gif
From Wikimedia Commons

The Monroe-Einstein illusion works by playing with the viewer’s expectations about the relationships between the features of the faces and the viewer’s sense of facial proportions.

Monroe-Einstein

Discovery of the Penrose Triangle Illusion

It was first created by the British mathematician and physicist Roger Penrose and his father Lionel Penrose, a psychiatrist and mathematician, in the 1950s.

Sir Roger Penrose is a renowned British mathematical physicist and Nobel laureate. He was born in 1931 and is still active today. He is known for his work in the fields of cosmology, general relativity, and quantum mechanics. He was awarded the Nobel Prize in Physics in 2020, jointly with Reinhard Genzel and Andrea Ghez, for his contributions to the understanding of the properties of black holes.

Penrose’s research has focused on the fundamental nature of space and time and the structure of the universe, he proposed new ways of understanding the Big Bang, black holes, and the nature of the universe itself. He has also been interested in the connection between physics and mathematics, particularly in the area of geometry. Penrose has written several books on these topics, which have been widely read and discussed by both scientists and the general public.

Penrose is also known for his work on the concept of “cosmic censorship” which is a theory that states that the singularities that occur in black holes are always hidden from the outside observer. He also developed new mathematical techniques, such as the Penrose diagram, which are used to describe the behavior of black holes and other singularities. Penrose is considered one of the most influential theoretical physicist of the 20th century and his work has had a significant impact on our understanding of the universe.

References and Resources

Check out our complete list of illusions.

Sign Up to Have Your Mind Blown