Zoetrope

Zoetrope

A zoetrope is a mechanical illusion device that creates the illusion of motion by rapidly displaying a sequence of still images in quick succession.

It consists of a cylinder with slits around the circumference and a series of still images placed inside. When the cylinder is rotated, the still images are viewed through the slits and appear to animate and move.

The zoetrope was one of the earliest forms of animation and was invented in the late 19th century. It was a precursor to motion picture technology and helped to lay the foundation for the development of cinema as we know it today. The zoetrope is considered a historical artifact and is often featured in museums and exhibitions showcasing the history of animation and visual effects.


Zoetrope
Zoetrope


Table of Contents

How does a Zoetrope work?

A zoetrope works by exploiting the persistence of vision, which is the phenomenon where the brain continues to see an image for a brief moment even after it has disappeared. When a zoetrope is rotated, the still images inside are viewed through the slits in quick succession. As the viewer looks through the slits, the brain combines the individual still images into a single, seamless moving image, creating the illusion of motion.

Here’s a step-by-step explanation of how a zoetrope works:

  1. A series of still images is placed inside the cylinder of the zoetrope.
  2. The cylinder is rotated at a high speed, so the still images are displayed through the slits in quick succession.
  3. As the viewer looks through the slits, the brain combines the individual still images into a single moving image, creating the illusion of motion.

The speed at which the cylinder is rotated and the frequency at which the still images are displayed through the slits are crucial to the success of the illusion. If the rotation is too slow or the still images are not displayed frequently enough, the illusion of motion will not be created.

Versions of a Zoetrope

The following are a few an alternates versions Zoetrope.


Zoetrope



Zoetrope



Illusions like the Zoetrope

A zoetrope is a type of mechanical optical illusion. It creates the illusion of motion by rapidly displaying a sequence of still images in quick succession, exploiting the persistence of vision phenomenon.

The zoetrope is considered an early form of animation and helped to lay the foundation for the development of cinema as we know it today. By creating the illusion of motion through the rapid display of still images, the zoetrope demonstrates how the brain processes visual information and creates the perception of motion.

The Zoetrope works by exploiting the persistence of vision, which is the phenomenon where the brain continues to perceive an image for a short period of time after it has been removed from view.

Persistence of vision is a phenomenon where the brain continues to perceive an image for a short period of time after it has been removed from view. This is due to the fact that the neurons in the visual system take a brief amount of time to process an image before they reset. As a result, if an image is presented rapidly enough, the brain will perceive a continuous image even though the individual images are no longer present.

For example, when watching a movie or animation, the individual frames are presented in rapid succession, creating the illusion of continuous motion. This is because the brain perceives the brief interval between each frame as a single, continuous image.

The persistence of vision phenomenon is important in many areas of visual perception, including motion perception, the perception of images in sequence, and the experience of visual illusions such as the Zoetrope.

Understanding the persistence of vision is essential for explaining the way that our visual system processes information and shapes our perception of the world around us.

Some related illusions include the following:

A Thaumatrope is a simple optical toy that was popular in the 19th century.

It consists of a disk with a picture or design on each side that is attached to two strings. When the strings are spun rapidly, the two pictures appear to merge into a single, continuous image.


Thaumatrope GIF


The Stepping Feet Illusion is a visual illusion that involves an optical illusion of the apparent movement of a static image.



The kinetic depth effect refers to the perception of three-dimensional (3D) depth and solidity in an object that is produced by its motion, relative to the observer.



The spinning dancer illusion is a visual illusion that depicts a silhouette of a dancer spinning clockwise or counterclockwise. The direction of the dancer’s spin can appear to change depending on the viewer’s perception.


The beta movement illusion is a visual illusion in which a series of static images, such as a filmstrip or flipbook, appears to be in motion when viewed in rapid succession.



In Peripheral Drift illusions, the image or pattern appears to move or drift, even though it is actually stationary. This movement is caused by the brain’s attempt to interpret the image or pattern, which is often complex or ambiguous. The movement can be in any direction, such as horizontally, vertically, or diagonally.



Peripheral drift illusion jelly bean
From Wikimedia Commons


The rotating snakes is a peripheral drift illusion that consists of a grid of shapes, with some of them appearing to be rotating or undulating. The illusion is created by the interaction of the shapes with the neural processing of the visual system.


Rotating Snakes Autokinetic effect
A Version of Rotating Snakes


With Illusory motion, static image appears to be moving due to the interaction of color contrasts, shapes, and position



Troxler’s fading, also known as Troxler’s effect, is a phenomenon in which a stationary visual stimulus eventually disappears from perception, even though it is still present in the visual field.



Lilac-Chaser_Troxlers Fading


The stroboscopic effect is a visual phenomenon that occurs when a rapidly flashing light is used to illuminate a moving object.

The object appears to be frozen in a single position, even though it is in motion. This is because the flashing light is only on for a brief period of time, and the object is only visible when the light is on.

Stroboscopic effect


The Scintillating Grid Illusion, in which a grid of black and white squares appears to pulsate or “breathe” when viewed from the periphery of the image.



Simultaneous Contrast Illusion


The Motion Aftereffect (also known as the Waterfall Illusion) is a visual illusion experienced after viewing a moving visual stimulus, and then fixating on a stationary stimulus. The stationary stimulus appears to move in the opposite direction to the original stimulus.

For example, fixate on the moving image below for several seconds, then shift you gaze to another surface like a plain wall. You should still be able to see the motion on the static surface.


Motion-Aftereffect



The Café Wall Illusion is a visual illusion that is created by a grid of alternating light and dark horizontal and vertical lines. The lines appear to be bent or tilted, even though they are actually straight.


Cafe Wall Illusion



Discovery of the Zoetrope

The zoetrope was created by William George Horner, a British mathematician and inventor, in 1834.

Horner was inspired by the phenomenon of persistence of vision and sought to create a device that would demonstrate how the brain processes visual information to create the perception of motion.

He came up with the idea of the zoetrope, a cylinder with slits around the circumference that displays a sequence of still images in quick succession to create the illusion of motion.

Horner’s invention was one of the earliest forms of animation and helped to lay the foundation for the development of cinema as we know it today.


William George Horner was a British mathematician and inventor born in 1786.

He is best known for inventing the zoetrope, a mechanical illusion device that creates the illusion of motion by rapidly displaying a sequence of still images in quick succession.

Horner was a prominent figure in the field of mathematics and was particularly interested in the study of optics and the way the human brain processes visual information.

His invention of the zoetrope demonstrated how the brain creates the perception of motion by combining individual still images into a single, seamless moving image.

Horner’s work was influential in the development of cinema and helped to lay the foundation for the motion picture industry.

He died in 1837

References and Resources


Check out our complete list of illusions.


Sign Up to Have Your Mind Blown


Vanishing Puzzle

A mechanical optical illusion where pieces which can be rearranged to show different versions that have different interpretations including more/less or smaller/larger pieces.

Vanishing PUzzle
Vanishing Puzzle


Table of Contents


How does the Vanishing Puzzle work??

The Vanishing puzzle works by exploiting the way the human visual system processes and interprets visual information.

Mechanical illusions are optical illusions that involve the use of moving parts or mechanisms to create the illusion. T

hey often rely on the viewer’s persistence of vision and the way our brains perceive motion. Some examples of mechanical illusions include the zoetrope, the praxinoscope, the phenakistoscope, and flip books. These illusions have been popular for centuries and continue to captivate audiences of all ages with their ability to trick the eye and create the illusion of movement.


Versions of the Vanishing Puzzle

The following is are some examples of Vanishing Puzzle designed to show how they work:

Missing Square Puzzle
Missing Square



Missing Square Puzzle
Missing Square Puzzle – Solution Shown



Illusions like the Vanishing Puzzle

Some related illusions include the following:

The missing square puzzle is a visual illusion that involves a square with a smaller square removed from one of its corners.

The puzzle is presented in such a way that the surrounding rectangles appear to be the same size, but when the lines of the squares are measured, it becomes clear that they are not.


Missing Square Puzzle
Missing Square Puzzle

Illusory contours are created by the brain’s interpretation of visual information, such as the arrangement and contrast of lines and shapes in the visual field.

Kanizsa Triangle Illusion
The Kanizsa Triangle

The Muller-Lyer illusion: an illusion in which two lines of the same length appear to be of different lengths due to the presence of arrowheads at the ends of the lines.


müller-lyer illusion



The Poggendorff illusion: an illusion in which two lines that are parallel in reality appear to be skewed or converging due to the presence of a third shape in the foreground.


Poggendorff illusion transparent gray bar



The Café Wall illusion: an illusion in which the lines that form a tiled wall appear to be skewed or distorted due to the presence of contrasting colored tiles.

Cafe Wall Illusion



The Zöllner illusion: an illusion in which parallel lines appear to be skewed or converging due to the presence of intersecting lines at an angle.

Zöllner illusion

The Ebbinghaus illusion: This illusion involves circles of different sizes, but with the same-sized circles placed close to each other. The brain perceives the smaller circles as being larger when they are placed next to larger circles.

Ebbinghaus Illusion


The T illusion: This illusion involves a T-shaped figure with a horizontal bar and a vertical bar. The brain perceives the vertical bar as being longer than the horizontal bar, even though they are the same length.


T Illusion


Moon illusion: This illusion involves the perception of the Moon appearing larger when it is near the horizon compared to when it is high in the sky.


Moon Illusion



Ponzo illusion: This illusion involves the perception of an object appearing larger or smaller depending on the size of the surrounding context.

Ponzo Illusion


All these illusions work by exploiting the way the brain processes visual information and the way it interprets the relationship between different elements in an image. Illusory contours are thought to be created by the activity of neurons that respond selectively to edges and contours in the visual field, which can be influenced by different visual cues.


Discovery of the Vanishing Puzzle

The origin of theVanishing Puzzle is unknown and its creator is not credited.

The puzzle has been widely used in psychology and neuroscience research to study visual perception and the underlying mechanisms of size constancy.

The puzzle has been used by researchers to investigate the importance of context in visual perception and to demonstrate the limitations of our visual system.

The Vanishing Puzzle is one of many optical illusions that have been used to further our understanding of how the brain processes and interprets visual information.

Size constancy illusions like the Missing Square Puzzle a form of the Vanishing Puzzle have been studied by a number of psychologists and neuroscientists, and the discovery of these illusions is often attributed to the work of Gestalt psychologists in the early 20th century.

Gestalt psychologists, such as Max Wertheimer, Kurt Koffka, and Wolfgang Köhler, were among the first to study how the brain processes and interprets visual information, and they made significant contributions to our understanding of size constancy.

They conducted a number of experiments that demonstrated the importance of context in visual perception and how our visual system can be easily influenced by the relationships between objects.

Their work laid the foundation for the continued study of size constancy illusions and the underlying mechanisms of visual perception.

References and Resources

Check out our complete list of illusions.

Sign Up to Have Your Mind Blown

Trompe-L’œil

Trompe-l'œil Capri

Trompe-l’œil is a French term that means “trick the eye.”

It refers to a style of art that uses realistic depictions of three-dimensional objects to create the illusion of depth and a life-like appearance.

Trompe-l’œil paintings and murals are often used to create the illusion of an extended space or to make a room appear larger than it actually is.

Trompe-l'œil Capri
Trompe-L’œil

Table of Contents

How does Trompe-L’œil work?

Trompe-l’œil works by creating the illusion of three-dimensional objects and space on a flat surface. The artist uses techniques such as perspective, shading, and texturing to create a highly realistic representation of the objects and to create the illusion of depth.

The techniques used in Trompe-l’œil take advantage of the way that the human visual system processes images. Our brains use cues such as size, shading, and perspective to determine the distance and depth of objects in our visual field. In Trompe-l’œil, the artist carefully manipulates these cues to create the illusion of depth and to make the objects appear to be physically present.

For example, in a Trompe-l’œil painting, the artist might use linear perspective to create the illusion of receding space. Objects that are closer to the viewer will appear larger, while objects that are further away will appear smaller. The artist will also use shading and texturing to create the illusion of depth and to make the objects appear more lifelike.

Trompe-l’œil works by tricking the brain into perceiving the two-dimensional image as a three-dimensional object, creating an illusion that is both fascinating and convincing. This is why Trompe-l’œil remains one of the most popular forms of visual illusion, and why it continues to be used today in various forms of art and design.

The technique of Trompe-l’œil has a long history, dating back to ancient Greece and Rome, and has been used throughout the centuries in various forms of art, including painting, sculpture, and architecture. In Trompe-l’œil, the artist uses a variety of techniques, such as perspective, shading, and texturing, to create the illusion of depth and to make the objects appear to be physically present.

Trompe-l’œil is often used in interior design, particularly for creating the illusion of an extended space or for creating a decorative feature on a wall or ceiling. It is also used in theatrical design, where it can be used to create the illusion of a larger stage or to create a realistic backdrop.

Trompe-l’œil is a popular art form that continues to be used today, and it remains one of the most fascinating and intriguing forms of visual illusion.

Versions of Trompe-L’œil

The following are some examples of Trompe-L’œil


Trompe-l'œil Copenhagen Zoo



Trompe-l'œil venice




Trompe-l'œil



Illusions like Trompe-L’œil

Trompe-l’œil is a type of optical illusion that creates the illusion of three-dimensional objects and space on a flat surface.

It falls under the category of visual perception illusions, which manipulate the way that the brain processes images to create an illusion that is different from reality. Trompe-l’œil is unique in that it uses realistic depictions of three-dimensional objects to create a highly convincing illusion of depth and physical presence.

Trompe-l’œil is often considered a form of representational art, as it creates a representation of three-dimensional objects. However, it is also a form of illusion, as the objects are not actually present in physical space, but are instead a two-dimensional representation that tricks the brain into perceiving them as three-dimensional.

Overall, Trompe-l’œil can be classified as a type of optical illusion, visual perception illusion, and representational art, depending on the context and the focus of the discussion.

Some similar illusions are as follows:

Forced perspective is a technique used in photography, architecture, and other visual arts to manipulate the perception of the size and distance of objects.

It creates the illusion of a larger or smaller object, or of one that is closer or farther away, by carefully controlling the angles, proportions, and placement of objects in the scene.

Forced Perspective
Forced Perspective

The Ponzo illusion is a type of visual illusion in which a pair of parallel lines, or a horizontal bar, appear to be of different lengths based on the presence of a converging or diverging set of lines, often resembling the converging parallel lines of a road or railway track, placed above and below them.

Ponzo illusion
The Ponzo Illusion

The Ebbinghaus illusion: This illusion is similar to the Delboeuf illusion but in this case the central circle appears larger when surrounded by smaller circles and smaller when surrounded by larger circles.

Ebbinghaus Illusion

The Delboeuf illusion is a perceptual illusion in which the perceived size of a circle is affected by the size of the surrounding circles.


The Occlusion illusion is a visual illusion where an object that is partially obscured by another object appears to be farther away than it actually is. This illusion is caused by the brain’s interpretation of the relative depth of the objects based on their relative size and position. This phenomenon is based on the visual cues that the brain uses to perceive depth, such as relative size, position, and overlap.

The occlusion illusion

The Müller-Lyer illusion is a classic example of size-contrast illusion, in which two lines of equal length appear to be different due to the presence of arrowheads or fins at the end of the lines.

müller-lyer illusion

The moon illusions involves the perception of the Moon appearing larger when it is near the horizon compared to when it is high in the sky.

The Kanizsa triangle is a visual illusion that was first described by the Italian psychologist Gaetano Kanizsa in 1955. The illusion consists of a white equilateral triangle that appears to be surrounded by three Pac-Man-like shapes, which are black and have white crescents facing inwards.

All these illusions demonstrate that the perceived size of an object can be influenced by the context in which it is presented, and that the brain uses the size of the surrounding objects as a reference point to judge the size of the central object.


Discovery of Trompe-L’œil

The exact origin of Trompe-l’œil as an art form is unclear, but it is thought to have been developed and popularized in ancient Greece and Rome. The technique was used in various forms throughout the centuries, and it was later adopted and refined by Renaissance artists in Italy.

It is difficult to attribute the discovery of Trompe-l’œil to a single individual, as the technique has a long history and has been used by many artists and cultures over the centuries. However, some of the most famous Trompe-l’œil artists from the Renaissance include Italian painters such as Andrea Pozzo, Giuseppe Maria Crespi, and Pietro da Cortona. These artists used Trompe-l’œil to create highly realistic murals and frescoes that adorned the ceilings and walls of churches, palaces, and private residences.

Overall, while no one person can be credited with discovering Trompe-l’œil, the art form has a rich and fascinating history that has been shaped and influenced by many artists and cultures over the centuries.

References and Resources

Check out our complete list of illusions.

Size-Weight Illusion

Size Weight Illusion

The “Size-Weight Illusion” is a perceptual phenomenon in which people perceive an object to be heavier when it is larger in size, even when the objects actually have the same weight.

This illusion occurs because our perceptions of size and weight are closely linked in our brain, and our expectations about the weight of an object are influenced by its size.

The Size-Weight Illusion, sometimes called the Charpentier illusion or the De Moor’s illusion, is thought to arise from the way our brain integrates sensory information from our hands, eyes, and other sources to form a coherent perception of the weight of an object. This information is then used to guide our actions and movements, such as lifting and carrying objects.

The “Size-Weight Illusion” has been extensively studied in the field of psychology and has been shown to be a robust phenomenon, occurring across a wide range of ages, cultures, and populations. It has practical implications in areas such as product design and ergonomics, where it is important to take into account the perception of size and weight when designing objects for human use.


Size Weight Illusion
Size-Weight Illusion


Table of Contents


How does the Size-Weight Illusion work?

The “Size-Weight Illusion” works by exploiting the close relationship between our perceptions of size and weight in the brain. The human brain integrates information from various sources, including touch and vision, to form a coherent perception of the weight of an object. When an object is larger in size, our brain unconsciously expects it to be heavier, and this expectation influences our perception of its weight.

The exact neural mechanisms underlying the “Size-Weight Illusion” are not completely understood, but some theories suggest that it may involve the activity of specialized brain regions that are responsible for processing information about object size, weight, and texture. These regions of the brain are thought to work together to generate a coherent perception of the weight of an object, and they may be influenced by factors such as past experience, expectations, and attention.

In summary, the “Size-Weight Illusion” works by exploiting the close relationship between our perceptions of size and weight in the brain, and by influencing our expectations about the weight of an object based on its size. This illusion demonstrates the complex and interrelated nature of our sensory experiences, and highlights the importance of the brain’s ability to integrate and process information from multiple sources to form a coherent perception of the world.


Versions of the Size-Weight Illusion

The following is an alternative Size-Weight Illusion example:



Size Weight Illusion



Illusions like the Size-Weight Illusion

The “Size-Weight Illusion” is a type of perceptual illusion. Perceptual illusions are instances in which our perceptions of the world around us are in conflict with the physical reality of the stimuli. In the case of the “Size-Weight Illusion,” our perception of the weight of an object is not in line with its actual weight, but is influenced by its size.

Perceptual illusions provide important insights into the workings of the human brain and how it processes sensory information to form our perceptions of the world. They demonstrate the complexity and interrelated nature of our sensory experiences and highlight the importance of the brain’s ability to integrate and process information from multiple sources to form a coherent perception of the world.

Overall, the “Size-Weight Illusion” is a type of perceptual illusion that demonstrates the close relationship between our perceptions of size and weight and highlights the importance of the brain’s ability to integrate and process information from multiple sources to form a coherent perception of the world.

Some related illusions include the following:

The missing square puzzle is a visual illusion that involves a square with a smaller square removed from one of its corners.

The puzzle is presented in such a way that the surrounding rectangles appear to be the same size, but when the lines of the squares are measured, it becomes clear that they are not.

The missing square puzzle is used to demonstrate the importance of context in visual perception and how our brain can be easily fooled into perceiving something that is not actually true.


Missing Square Puzzle
Missing Square Puzzle


Illusory contours are created by the brain’s interpretation of visual information, such as the arrangement and contrast of lines and shapes in the visual field.

Kanizsa Triangle Illusion
The Kanizsa Triangle

The Muller-Lyer illusion: an illusion in which two lines of the same length appear to be of different lengths due to the presence of arrowheads at the ends of the lines.


müller-lyer illusion



The Poggendorff illusion: an illusion in which two lines that are parallel in reality appear to be skewed or converging due to the presence of a third shape in the foreground.


Poggendorff illusion transparent gray bar



The Café Wall illusion: an illusion in which the lines that form a tiled wall appear to be skewed or distorted due to the presence of contrasting colored tiles.

Cafe Wall Illusion



The Zöllner illusion: an illusion in which parallel lines appear to be skewed or converging due to the presence of intersecting lines at an angle.

Zöllner illusion

The Ebbinghaus illusion: This illusion involves circles of different sizes, but with the same-sized circles placed close to each other. The brain perceives the smaller circles as being larger when they are placed next to larger circles.

Ebbinghaus Illusion


The T illusion: This illusion involves a T-shaped figure with a horizontal bar and a vertical bar. The brain perceives the vertical bar as being longer than the horizontal bar, even though they are the same length.


T Illusion


Moon illusion: This illusion involves the perception of the Moon appearing larger when it is near the horizon compared to when it is high in the sky.


Moon Illusion



Ponzo illusion: This illusion involves the perception of an object appearing larger or smaller depending on the size of the surrounding context.

Ponzo Illusion


All these illusions work by exploiting the way the brain processes visual information and the way it interprets the relationship between different elements in an image. Illusory contours are thought to be created by the activity of neurons that respond selectively to edges and contours in the visual field, which can be influenced by different visual cues.


Discovery of the Size-Weight Illusion

The “Size-Weight Illusion” was first documented by a psychologist named Ernst Heinrich Weber in the late 18th century. Weber was one of the earliest researchers to systematically study the relationship between size, weight, and perceived weight, and his work laid the foundation for the modern field of psychophysics.

In his experiments, Weber asked participants to judge the weight of objects of different sizes and found that larger objects were consistently rated as being heavier than smaller objects of the same weight. This observation led him to conclude that there was a close relationship between size and perceived weight and that this relationship was influenced by expectations and previous experiences.

Since Weber’s original observations, the “Size-Weight Illusion” has been extensively studied and replicated by researchers in the fields of psychology and neuroscience. Today, it is considered a well-established phenomenon and is widely recognized as a classic example of a perceptual illusion.

The illusion is sometimes called the Charpentier illusion, after the French physician Augustin Charpentier because he was the first to demonstrate the illusion experimentally. It is also called De Moor’s illusion, after Belgian physician Jean Demoor.

References and Resources

Check out our complete list of illusions.

Sign Up to Have Your Mind Blown

Anamorphic Street Art

Anamorphic street art

Anamorphic street art is a form of street art that uses optical illusion to create a three-dimensional image when viewed from a specific angle.

Anamorphic street art is often created by distorting the image, so that when it is viewed from a specific viewpoint, the image appears to be three-dimensional and in full perspective.

It is often seen as a way of transforming urban spaces into playful, interactive environments.

Anamorphic street art
Anamorphic Street Art



Table of Contents


How does Anamorphic Street Art?

Anamorphic street art works by manipulating the way our eyes perceive the image.

The artist distorts the image in such a way that it appears to be in the correct perspective when viewed from a specific viewpoint, typically at a specific angle or from a certain distance.

The use of perspective and forced-perspective techniques tricks the viewer’s eye into perceiving a three-dimensional image where there is actually only a flat surface.

The result is an optical illusion that creates the illusion of depth and volume, making the image appear to pop out of the wall or floor.

To create an anamorphic street art piece, the artist needs to carefully plan the placement of the artwork and the viewpoint from which it will be viewed to ensure the illusion is successful.

Versions of Anamorphic Street Art

The following are other examples of Anamorphic Street Art



Phantograms



Phantograms




Phantograms



Illusions like Anamorphic Street Art

Anamorphic Street Art are a form of stereograms, which are images that use the difference between the left and right eye views to create the illusion of depth and 3D objects.

Anamorphic street art is a type of perspective illusion that creates the illusion of a three-dimensional image on a two-dimensional surface by manipulating the way our eyes perceive the image.

The image is distorted in such a way that when viewed from a specific angle or distance, it appears to be in correct perspective and the illusion of depth and volume is created.

The illusion is achieved by using techniques such as forced perspective and manipulation of perspective, which trick the viewer’s eye into perceiving the image as three-dimensional.

Some related illusions include the following:

An autostereogram is a type of image that appears to be a flat 2D image when viewed normally, but when viewed with a special technique, it appears to be a 3D image with depth and perspective.

Autostereograms are created by repeating a pattern of repeating elements, such as random dots, in such a way that the repeating elements at different depths in the image align with each other when viewed with the special technique. This creates the illusion of a 3D image.

The image below appears as a 2 dimensional flat image, but when viewed using one of the techniques mentioned below, a 3 dimensional shape appears.


Phantograms are 3D images that appear to float in space and can be viewed without special glasses or other aids. The term “Phantogram” is derived from the Greek words “phaneros,” meaning “visible,” and “gramma,” meaning “something written or drawn.”

Phantograms are created by taking two photos of an object from slightly different angles and then printing the images on a flat surface, such as a piece of paper or card. The two images are then viewed together, and the slight differences in perspective create the illusion of depth and the appearance of a floating 3D object.


Persistence of vision is the phenomenon by which the brain continues to perceive an image even after the image is no longer present.

This occurs because the cells in the retina, called rods and cones, take a brief period of time to “reset” after being stimulated.



A color constancy illusion is a type of visual illusion in which a color appears to be different when viewed in different contexts.

For example, the same patch of color may appear lighter or darker when viewed against different backgrounds, or may appear to change color when viewed under different lighting conditions.


Color constancy
Color Constancy


The afterimage illusion is a type of visual illusion in which an image continues to appear in the observer’s visual field after the original stimulus has been removed.

This can occur due to the persistence of neural activity in the visual system, and can take the form of a positive afterimage (an image that is the same color as the original stimulus) or a negative afterimage (an image that is the opposite color of the original stimulus).



The Checker Shadow Illusion is created by a checkerboard pattern composed of squares with different luminance values, the squares that are not directly illuminated by the light source appear darker than the illuminated squares, creating the illusion of shadows.


Edelson-Checker_shadow_illusion
Checker Shadow Illusion


The simultaneous contrast illusion is a visual effect that occurs when the perception of a color is affected by the colors of the surrounding area.

The illusion creates the appearance of a change in the color of an object, even though the actual color of the object remains constant.


Simultaneous Contrast Effect
The center green dot is the same on both sides, but the surrounding color changes the perception


The Neon Color Spreading illusion refers to the visual phenomenon where an area of color appears to spread or “bleed” beyond its intended boundaries.

Neon Color Spreading
From Wikimedia Commons


The Bezold Effect: This illusion is created by placing two or more colors next to each other, and the way they appear to change when they are close to one another.  


Bezold_Effect
from Wikimedia Commons


The Cornsweet illusion is a classic example of a brightness illusion, which is an illusion in which two areas that are physically the same brightness appear to be different in brightness.

Cornsweet illusion
Cornsweet illusion


The Chubb illusion is based on the perception of brightness and can be observed when a small bright patch is surrounded by a larger dark area, the small bright patch will appear brighter than the same patch surrounded by a bright area.


Chubb Illusion


White’s illusion is a visual phenomenon in which two identical gray bars are placed on a background of alternating black and white stripes.

The gray bars appear to be different shades of gray, with the one on the white stripes appearing lighter than the one on the black stripes.

In the image below, both gray bars have the exact same color.


White's Illusion
White’s Illusion

The Watercolor Illusion: This illusion is created by the way the brain perceives edges of an object. When an object is surrounded by a colored halo, the object appears to have a different color than it actually does.

Watercolor Illusion



The Café Wall Illusion is a visual illusion that is created by a grid of alternating light and dark horizontal and vertical lines. The lines appear to be bent or tilted, even though they are actually straight.


Cafe Wall Illusion


Discovery of Anamorphic Street Art

The origin of anamorphic street art is not well documented, and it’s unclear who created the first piece of anamorphic street art.

However, the use of anamorphosis in art dates back to the Renaissance, where artists such as Leonardo da Vinci and Albrecht Dürer used the technique to create illusions of depth in their drawings and paintings.

In the modern era, street artists around the world have been inspired by these earlier techniques and have started incorporating anamorphic illusions into their street art, turning urban spaces into interactive, playful environments.

Some well-known street artists who have used anamorphic techniques in their work include Eduardo Sanson, Eduardo Relero, and Felipe Pantone, among others.

References and Resources

Check out our complete list of illusions.

Sign Up to Have Your Mind Blown

Phantograms

Phantograms

Phantograms are 3D images that appear to float in space and can be viewed without special glasses or other aids. The term “Phantogram” is derived from the Greek words “phaneros,” meaning “visible,” and “gramma,” meaning “something written or drawn.”

Phantograms are created by taking two photos of an object from slightly different angles and then printing the images on a flat surface, such as a piece of paper or card. The two images are then viewed together, and the slight differences in perspective create the illusion of depth and the appearance of a floating 3D object.

Phantograms can be created using specialized software or by hand and can be viewed by simply holding the printed image at the correct angle and looking at it with both eyes. They have been used in a variety of applications, including art, advertising, and education.

3D street art is one of the most well known types of Phantogram. Both 3D street art and Phantograms use the principle of perspective to create the illusion of depth and a three-dimensional appearance. 3D street art, also known as anamorphic street art, is created by painting an image on a flat surface in such a way that it appears to be three-dimensional when viewed from a specific angle.


Phantograms
3D Street Art – Phantogram Example



Table of Contents


How do Phantograms work?

Phantograms are 2D images that create the illusion of a 3D object or scene when viewed from a specific angle.

They work by manipulating the spatial relationship between the foreground and background elements in an image.

The foreground elements are arranged to converge towards a vanishing point and are distorted to give the illusion of depth, while the background elements are left undistorted.

When viewed from the intended angle, the image appears to pop out from the flat surface.

The effect is created by the interaction between the visual information in the image and the brain’s perception of depth and perspective.

Versions of the Phantograms

The following are other examples of Phantograms including a few 3D street examples and a classic image of a phantogram with chess pieces.



Phantograms



Phantograms




Phantograms





Illusions like Phantograms

Phantograms are a form of stereograms, which are images that use the difference between the left and right eye views to create the illusion of depth and 3D objects. In other words, phantograms are a type of optical illusion that tricks the brain into perceiving a 2D image as a 3D scene.

Some related illusions include the following:

An autostereogram is a type of image that appears to be a flat 2D image when viewed normally, but when viewed with a special technique, it appears to be a 3D image with depth and perspective.

Autostereograms are created by repeating a pattern of repeating elements, such as random dots, in such a way that the repeating elements at different depths in the image align with each other when viewed with the special technique. This creates the illusion of a 3D image.

The image below appears as a 2 dimensional flat image, but when viewed using one of the techniques mentioned below, a 3 dimensional shape appears.



Persistence of vision is the phenomenon by which the brain continues to perceive an image even after the image is no longer present.

This occurs because the cells in the retina, called rods and cones, take a brief period of time to “reset” after being stimulated.



A color constancy illusion is a type of visual illusion in which a color appears to be different when viewed in different contexts.

For example, the same patch of color may appear lighter or darker when viewed against different backgrounds, or may appear to change color when viewed under different lighting conditions.


Color constancy
Color Constancy


The afterimage illusion is a type of visual illusion in which an image continues to appear in the observer’s visual field after the original stimulus has been removed.

This can occur due to the persistence of neural activity in the visual system, and can take the form of a positive afterimage (an image that is the same color as the original stimulus) or a negative afterimage (an image that is the opposite color of the original stimulus).



The Checker Shadow Illusion is created by a checkerboard pattern composed of squares with different luminance values, the squares that are not directly illuminated by the light source appear darker than the illuminated squares, creating the illusion of shadows.


Edelson-Checker_shadow_illusion
Checker Shadow Illusion


The simultaneous contrast illusion is a visual effect that occurs when the perception of a color is affected by the colors of the surrounding area.

The illusion creates the appearance of a change in the color of an object, even though the actual color of the object remains constant.


Simultaneous Contrast Effect
The center green dot is the same on both sides, but the surrounding color changes the perception


The Neon Color Spreading illusion refers to the visual phenomenon where an area of color appears to spread or “bleed” beyond its intended boundaries.

Neon Color Spreading
From Wikimedia Commons


The Bezold Effect: This illusion is created by placing two or more colors next to each other, and the way they appear to change when they are close to one another.  


Bezold_Effect
from Wikimedia Commons


The Cornsweet illusion is a classic example of a brightness illusion, which is an illusion in which two areas that are physically the same brightness appear to be different in brightness.

Cornsweet illusion
Cornsweet illusion


The Chubb illusion is based on the perception of brightness and can be observed when a small bright patch is surrounded by a larger dark area, the small bright patch will appear brighter than the same patch surrounded by a bright area.


Chubb Illusion


White’s illusion is a visual phenomenon in which two identical gray bars are placed on a background of alternating black and white stripes.

The gray bars appear to be different shades of gray, with the one on the white stripes appearing lighter than the one on the black stripes.

In the image below, both gray bars have the exact same color.


White's Illusion
White’s Illusion

The Watercolor Illusion: This illusion is created by the way the brain perceives edges of an object. When an object is surrounded by a colored halo, the object appears to have a different color than it actually does.

Watercolor Illusion



The Café Wall Illusion is a visual illusion that is created by a grid of alternating light and dark horizontal and vertical lines. The lines appear to be bent or tilted, even though they are actually straight.


Cafe Wall Illusion


Discovery of the Phantograms

The origin of phantograms is not attributed to any single person or group.

The technique has been used for many centuries in various forms of art and design, and the basic principles behind phantograms and stereoscopy have been understood and described by artists, scientists, and mathematicians for a long time.

However, the term “phantogram” itself may have been coined more recently and its usage and definition have evolved over time in the field of photography and graphic design.

References and Resources

Check out our complete list of illusions.

Sign Up to Have Your Mind Blown

Pepper’s Ghost

Peppers Ghost

Pepper’s Ghost is an optical illusion used in theater and theme parks to create ghostly or translucent apparitions.

The illusion works by reflecting an object, usually a person, onto a transparent sheet (often made of glass or plastic) at a 45-degree angle, making it appear as if the object is floating in the air.

The transparent sheet is carefully positioned between the audience and the stage, making the reflection appear as a transparent ghost-like figure to the audience.

Peppers Ghost
Peppers Ghost

Table of Contents


How does Peppers Ghost work?

Pepper’s Ghost works by using a combination of reflection and perspective to create the illusion of a ghostly or translucent figure. Here’s how it works:

1: A partially transparent sheet of glass or plastic is placed at a 45-degree angle between the audience and the stage.

2: A hidden room or area is created behind the transparent sheet, where the object to be reflected is positioned. This could be a live actor or a physical object.

3: Bright lights are shone on the object, causing its reflection to be visible on the transparent sheet.

4: The audience sees the reflection on the transparent sheet as if it is floating in the air, due to the angle at which the sheet is positioned. The angle also helps to obscure the actual source of the reflection.

5) By carefully controlling the lighting and positioning of the object in the hidden room, the illusion can be made to appear and disappear, or even change appearance, creating a convincing ghostly or translucent effect.

This illusion was popularized by John Henry Pepper, a British scientist and inventor, who first demonstrated the effect in the 19th century.


Versions of Peppers Ghost

The following are some additional images that help show how Peppers Ghost works:

Peppers Ghost 1
If looking through the red rectangle, you see a ghost floating next to the table. The green outline represents the Plexiglas. The room to the left is hidden from the viewer and is a mirror image of the right side


Peppers Ghost 2
If the mirror-image room (left) is darkened, it does not reflect well in the glass. The empty room (top) is brightly lit, making it very visible to the viewer

Peppers Ghost
When the lights in the mirror-image room are raised (with the empty room being dimmed slightly to compensate), the ghost appears out of nowhere


Illusions like Peppers Ghost

Pepper’s Ghost is a reflective illusion. It creates the appearance of a ghostly or translucent figure by reflecting an object onto a transparent sheet and positioning it in such a way that the reflection appears to the audience as if it is floating in the air. The transparent sheet acts as a reflector, and the 45-degree angle at which it is positioned helps to obscure the actual source of the reflection and enhances the illusion.

Some examples other illusions that exploit angles and positioning include:

The Kanizsa triangle is a visual illusion that was first described by the Italian psychologist Gaetano Kanizsa in 1955. The illusion consists of a white equilateral triangle that appears to be surrounded by three Pac-Man-like shapes, which are black and have white crescents facing inwards.

Kanizsa Triangle Illusion
The Kanizsa Triangle

The Muller-Lyer illusion: an illusion in which two lines of the same length appear to be of different lengths due to the presence of arrowheads at the ends of the lines.


müller-lyer illusion



The Poggendorff illusion: an illusion in which two lines that are parallel in reality appear to be skewed or converging due to the presence of a third shape in the foreground.


Poggendorff illusion transparent gray bar



The Café Wall illusion: an illusion in which the lines that form a tiled wall appear to be skewed or distorted due to the presence of contrasting colored tiles.

Cafe Wall Illusion



The Zöllner illusion: an illusion in which parallel lines appear to be skewed or converging due to the presence of intersecting lines at an angle.

Zöllner illusion

The Ebbinghaus illusion: This illusion involves circles of different sizes, but with the same-sized circles placed close to each other. The brain perceives the smaller circles as being larger when they are placed next to larger circles.

Ebbinghaus Illusion


The T illusion: This illusion involves a T-shaped figure with a horizontal bar and a vertical bar. The brain perceives the vertical bar as being longer than the horizontal bar, even though they are the same length.


T Illusion


Moon illusion: This illusion involves the perception of the Moon appearing larger when it is near the horizon compared to when it is high in the sky.


Moon Illusion



Ponzo illusion: This illusion involves the perception of an object appearing larger or smaller depending on the size of the surrounding context.

Ponzo Illusion


All these illusions work by exploiting the way the brain processes visual information and the way it interprets the relationship between different elements in an image. Illusory contours are thought to be created by the activity of neurons that respond selectively to edges and contours in the visual field, which can be influenced by different visual cues.


Discovery of Peppers Ghost

This illusion was popularized by John Henry Pepper, a British scientist and inventor, who first demonstrated the effect in the 19th century.

John Henry Pepper was a British scientist and inventor who lived from 1821 to 1900.

He is best known for popularizing the Pepper’s Ghost illusion, a type of optical illusion that creates the appearance of ghostly or translucent figures using reflections and perspective.

Pepper first demonstrated the effect in a stage show in the 1860s and it quickly became popular as a theatrical effect.

The illusion has since been used in a variety of settings, including theme parks, museums, and haunted houses.

Pepper’s contributions to the field of optics and illusion have earned him a place in the history of entertainment technology.

References and Resources

Check out our complete list of illusions.

Sign Up to Have Your Mind Blown

Numerosity Adaptation Effect

Numerosity adaptation effect

The Numerosity adaptation effect refers to a phenomenon in which the perceived number of objects in a display is altered after exposure to a differently sized display.

The effect occurs when an individual is presented with a display of objects with a specific number density (number of objects per unit area) and then presented with a display of a different number density.

After exposure to the differently sized display, the individual’s perception of the number of objects in a subsequent display is altered and biased towards the number density of the adapted display.

The Numerosity adaptation effect is a demonstration of the plasticity of the visual system and how it can be influenced by experience. The effect has been widely studied in psychology and neuroscience and has been used to further our understanding of the underlying mechanisms of numerical perception and the role of experience in shaping perception.


Numerosity adaptation effect
Numerosity Adaptation Effect
From Wikimedia Commons


Table of Contents


How does the Numerosity Adaptation Effect work?

The Numerosity adaptation effect works by altering the sensitivity of the visual system to changes in the number density of objects in a display.

When an individual is exposed to a display of objects with a specific number density, the visual system adapts to that number density and becomes more sensitive to changes in that particular number density.

This leads to a bias in the perception of subsequent displays towards the number density of the adapted display.

The effect is thought to result from changes in the responsiveness of neurons in the visual cortex that are involved in representing the number of objects in a display.

The exact mechanism by which the Numerosity adaptation effect works is not well understood, but it is thought to involve changes in the gain or sensitivity of neural populations that are involved in processing numerical information.

The effect is thought to reflect a form of neural plasticity, where the visual system adapts to the statistics of the environment and changes the responsiveness of neurons accordingly.

This leads to a recalibration of the visual system’s sensitivity to changes in numerosity and a bias in the perception of subsequent displays.


Versions of the Numerosity Adaptation Effect

The following is an alternative Numerosity Adaptation Effect example:



Numerosity adaptation effect
Effect of Random vs. Organized Dots
All Circles on Left have the Same Number
All Circles on Right have the Same Number



Illusions like the Numerosity Adaptation Effect

The Numerosity adaptation effect is a type of perceptual illusion.

It is a demonstration of how our perception of the world can be influenced by experience and context. Perceptual illusions are situations where the sensory information that is being processed by the brain does not accurately reflect the physical reality of the stimulus.

The Numerosity adaptation effect is a form of illusion because it results in a misperception of the number of objects in a display, even though the physical number of objects has not changed. The effect is a result of changes in the responsiveness of neurons in the visual cortex that are involved in processing numerical information, and it reflects the plasticity and adaptability of the visual system.

Some related illusions include the following:

The missing square puzzle is a visual illusion that involves a square with a smaller square removed from one of its corners.

The puzzle is presented in such a way that the surrounding rectangles appear to be the same size, but when the lines of the squares are measured, it becomes clear that they are not.

The missing square puzzle is used to demonstrate the importance of context in visual perception and how our brain can be easily fooled into perceiving something that is not actually true.


Missing Square Puzzle
Missing Square Puzzle


Illusory contours are created by the brain’s interpretation of visual information, such as the arrangement and contrast of lines and shapes in the visual field.

Kanizsa Triangle Illusion
The Kanizsa Triangle

The Muller-Lyer illusion: an illusion in which two lines of the same length appear to be of different lengths due to the presence of arrowheads at the ends of the lines.


müller-lyer illusion



The Poggendorff illusion: an illusion in which two lines that are parallel in reality appear to be skewed or converging due to the presence of a third shape in the foreground.


Poggendorff illusion transparent gray bar



The Café Wall illusion: an illusion in which the lines that form a tiled wall appear to be skewed or distorted due to the presence of contrasting colored tiles.

Cafe Wall Illusion



The Zöllner illusion: an illusion in which parallel lines appear to be skewed or converging due to the presence of intersecting lines at an angle.

Zöllner illusion

The Ebbinghaus illusion: This illusion involves circles of different sizes, but with the same-sized circles placed close to each other. The brain perceives the smaller circles as being larger when they are placed next to larger circles.

Ebbinghaus Illusion


The T illusion: This illusion involves a T-shaped figure with a horizontal bar and a vertical bar. The brain perceives the vertical bar as being longer than the horizontal bar, even though they are the same length.


T Illusion


Moon illusion: This illusion involves the perception of the Moon appearing larger when it is near the horizon compared to when it is high in the sky.


Moon Illusion



Ponzo illusion: This illusion involves the perception of an object appearing larger or smaller depending on the size of the surrounding context.

Ponzo Illusion


All these illusions work by exploiting the way the brain processes visual information and the way it interprets the relationship between different elements in an image. Illusory contours are thought to be created by the activity of neurons that respond selectively to edges and contours in the visual field, which can be influenced by different visual cues.


Discovery of the Numerosity Adaptation Effect

The Numerosity adaptation effect was first documented in the psychological and neuroscience literature in the 1990s and has since been widely studied. It has been covered in some detail by Stanislas Dehaene.

The exact origin of the effect and who first discovered it is unclear, as multiple studies and researchers have contributed to our understanding of the effect.

The Numerosity adaptation effect is a result of the collective efforts of psychologists, neuroscientists, and other researchers who have worked to better understand the mechanisms of numerical perception and the role of experience in shaping perception.

The effect is a demonstration of the dynamic and adaptable nature of the visual system, and its discovery has helped to further our understanding of the underlying mechanisms of visual perception.

References and Resources

Check out our complete list of illusions.

Sign Up to Have Your Mind Blown

Missing Square Puzzle

Missing Square Puzzle

The missing square puzzle is a visual illusion that involves a square with a smaller square removed from one of its corners.

The puzzle is presented in such a way that the surrounding rectangles appear to be the same size, but when the lines of the squares are measured, it becomes clear that they are not.

The missing square puzzle is used to demonstrate the importance of context in visual perception and how our brain can be easily fooled into perceiving something that is not actually true.

It also highlights the limitations of our visual system and how it can be influenced by our assumptions and expectations.


Missing Square Puzzle
Missing Square Puzzle


Table of Contents


How does the Missing Square Puzzle?

The missing square puzzle works by exploiting the way the human visual system processes and interprets visual information.

The brain relies on various cues, such as perspective, shading, and the relationships between objects, to make sense of the visual world.

In the missing square puzzle, the brain uses these cues to perceive the surrounding rectangles as being the same size, even though they are not.

The illusion works because the brain is influenced by the contextual information provided by the surrounding rectangles and their relative size, rather than relying solely on the actual measurements of the squares.

This leads to a misperception of the size of the squares and the overall shape of the puzzle, creating the illusion of a missing square.


Versions of the Missing Square Puzzle

The following is an alternative Missing Square Puzzle:



Missing Square Puzzle
Missing Square



Missing Square Puzzle
Missing Square Puzzle – Solution Shown



Illusions like the Missing Square Puzzle

The missing square puzzle is a type of size-constancy illusion.

Size-constancy illusions refer to situations where the perceived size of an object remains constant, even though its physical size changes.

In the missing square puzzle, the perceived size of the surrounding rectangles remains constant, even though their physical size changes.

The illusion is a demonstration of how our visual system processes and interprets visual information and how it can be influenced by contextual cues, such as the relative size of surrounding objects.

The missing square puzzle is also sometimes referred to as an optical illusion or perceptual illusion.

Some related illusions include the following:

Illusory contours are created by the brain’s interpretation of visual information, such as the arrangement and contrast of lines and shapes in the visual field.

Kanizsa Triangle Illusion
The Kanizsa Triangle

The Muller-Lyer illusion: an illusion in which two lines of the same length appear to be of different lengths due to the presence of arrowheads at the ends of the lines.


müller-lyer illusion



The Poggendorff illusion: an illusion in which two lines that are parallel in reality appear to be skewed or converging due to the presence of a third shape in the foreground.


Poggendorff illusion transparent gray bar



The Café Wall illusion: an illusion in which the lines that form a tiled wall appear to be skewed or distorted due to the presence of contrasting colored tiles.

Cafe Wall Illusion



The Zöllner illusion: an illusion in which parallel lines appear to be skewed or converging due to the presence of intersecting lines at an angle.

Zöllner illusion

The Ebbinghaus illusion: This illusion involves circles of different sizes, but with the same-sized circles placed close to each other. The brain perceives the smaller circles as being larger when they are placed next to larger circles.

Ebbinghaus Illusion


The T illusion: This illusion involves a T-shaped figure with a horizontal bar and a vertical bar. The brain perceives the vertical bar as being longer than the horizontal bar, even though they are the same length.


T Illusion


Moon illusion: This illusion involves the perception of the Moon appearing larger when it is near the horizon compared to when it is high in the sky.


Moon Illusion



Ponzo illusion: This illusion involves the perception of an object appearing larger or smaller depending on the size of the surrounding context.

Ponzo Illusion


All these illusions work by exploiting the way the brain processes visual information and the way it interprets the relationship between different elements in an image. Illusory contours are thought to be created by the activity of neurons that respond selectively to edges and contours in the visual field, which can be influenced by different visual cues.


Discovery of the Missing Square Puzzle

The origin of the missing square puzzle is unknown and its creator is not credited.

The puzzle has been widely used in psychology and neuroscience research to study visual perception and the underlying mechanisms of size constancy.

The puzzle has been used by researchers to investigate the importance of context in visual perception and to demonstrate the limitations of our visual system.

The missing square puzzle is one of many optical illusions that have been used to further our understanding of how the brain processes and interprets visual information.

Size constancy illusions like the Missing Square Puzzle have been studied by a number of psychologists and neuroscientists, and the discovery of these illusions is often attributed to the work of Gestalt psychologists in the early 20th century.

Gestalt psychologists, such as Max Wertheimer, Kurt Koffka, and Wolfgang Köhler, were among the first to study how the brain processes and interprets visual information, and they made significant contributions to our understanding of size constancy.

They conducted a number of experiments that demonstrated the importance of context in visual perception and how our visual system can be easily influenced by the relationships between objects.

Their work laid the foundation for the continued study of size constancy illusions and the underlying mechanisms of visual perception.

References and Resources

Check out our complete list of illusions.

Sign Up to Have Your Mind Blown

Illusory Contours

Illusory Contour

Illusory contours are visual stimuli that appear to form a boundary or shape, but in reality do not exist as a solid object.

Illusory contours are created by the brain’s interpretation of visual information, such as the arrangement and contrast of lines and shapes in the visual field.

The brain perceives these stimuli as a solid edge or contour, even though no physical boundary is present. Illusory contours are a well-known phenomenon in the field of visual perception, and are often used to study the brain’s interpretation of visual information.

Illusory Contour
Illusory Contours – Ehrenstein Figure

Table of Contents


How do Illusory Contours work?

Illusory contours work by exploiting the brain’s interpretation of visual information.

The brain processes information about the arrangement, contrast, and proximity of lines and shapes in the visual field, and uses this information to form a perception of solid objects and boundaries.

When lines and shapes are arranged in a way that suggests a boundary or contour, the brain perceives this as a solid edge, even though no physical boundary is present.

This is the basis for illusory contours.

Illusory contours can be created by manipulating factors such as the arrangement, contrast, and proximity of lines and shapes, as well as the position of the observer relative to the stimuli.

The brain’s interpretation of these visual cues leads to the perception of a solid edge or contour, even though no physical boundary is present.

The process of creating and perceiving illusory contours is a complex interaction between the visual system and the brain, and continues to be the subject of much research and study.


Versions of Illusory Contours

The following are some alternate versions of Illusory Contours:

Illusory Contour
Kanizsa-Star
Source: Wikimedia Commons
Reification-Examples-Kanizsa
Source: Wikimedia Commons


Kanizsa-Square
Kanizsa-Triangle-colorful


Illusions like Illusory Contours

Illusory contour illusions are a type of visual illusion in which the brain perceives contours or edges that are not physically present in an image.

These illusions can be created by using a variety of visual cues, such as shading, texture, or the arrangement of simple shapes, to create the illusion of a more complex shape.

Some examples of illusions like illusory contours include:

The Kanizsa triangle is a visual illusion that was first described by the Italian psychologist Gaetano Kanizsa in 1955. The illusion consists of a white equilateral triangle that appears to be surrounded by three Pac-Man-like shapes, which are black and have white crescents facing inwards.

The three Pac-Man-like shapes are actually made up of three partially-overlapping circles with white edges that create the illusion of a triangle

Kanizsa Triangle Illusion
The Kanizsa Triangle

The Muller-Lyer illusion: an illusion in which two lines of the same length appear to be of different lengths due to the presence of arrowheads at the ends of the lines.


müller-lyer illusion



The Poggendorff illusion: an illusion in which two lines that are parallel in reality appear to be skewed or converging due to the presence of a third shape in the foreground.


Poggendorff illusion transparent gray bar



The Café Wall illusion: an illusion in which the lines that form a tiled wall appear to be skewed or distorted due to the presence of contrasting colored tiles.

Cafe Wall Illusion



The Zöllner illusion: an illusion in which parallel lines appear to be skewed or converging due to the presence of intersecting lines at an angle.

Zöllner illusion

The Ebbinghaus illusion: This illusion involves circles of different sizes, but with the same-sized circles placed close to each other. The brain perceives the smaller circles as being larger when they are placed next to larger circles.

Ebbinghaus Illusion


The T illusion: This illusion involves a T-shaped figure with a horizontal bar and a vertical bar. The brain perceives the vertical bar as being longer than the horizontal bar, even though they are the same length.


T Illusion


Moon illusion: This illusion involves the perception of the Moon appearing larger when it is near the horizon compared to when it is high in the sky.


Moon Illusion



Ponzo illusion: This illusion involves the perception of an object appearing larger or smaller depending on the size of the surrounding context.

Ponzo Illusion


All these illusions work by exploiting the way the brain processes visual information and the way it interprets the relationship between different elements in an image. Illusory contours are thought to be created by the activity of neurons that respond selectively to edges and contours in the visual field, which can be influenced by different visual cues.


Discovery of Illusory Contours

The discovery of illusory contours is not credited to a specific person or group.

The concept of illusory contours has been known and studied in the field of psychology and visual perception for many years. T

he phenomenon of illusory contours has been described and analyzed in numerous research papers, and is widely recognized as a fundamental aspect of visual perception.

It has been studied by many researchers, including psychologists, neuroscientists, and vision scientists, and continues to be the subject of much research and investigation.

On of the most famous illusory contours, the Kanizsa triangle, was first described by the Italian psychologist Gaetano Kanizsa in 1955.

Gaetano Kanizsa (1913-1993) was an Italian psychologist and neuropsychologist, known for his research in the field of visual perception and his contributions to the understanding of visual illusions. He is most famous for his work on amodal completion, which refers to the brain’s ability to fill in missing information to complete an object or shape, and for his description of the Kanizsa triangle, one of the most famous and studied amodal completion illusion.

He was a professor of Psychology at the University of Trieste in Italy, and his research focused on the relationship between perception and reality. He also developed the concept of “subjective contours” which refers to the ability of the brain to perceive contours that are not physically present in an image, and was a pioneer in the field of neuropsychology. His work has had a significant impact on the field of cognitive psychology and has influenced the work of many researchers in the field of visual perception.

References and Resources

Check out our complete list of illusions.

Sign Up to Have Your Mind Blown